Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Perceptual symbol systems.Lawrence W. Barsalou - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4):577-660.
    Prior to the twentieth century, theories of knowledge were inherently perceptual. Since then, developments in logic, statis- tics, and programming languages have inspired amodal theories that rest on principles fundamentally different from those underlying perception. In addition, perceptual approaches have become widely viewed as untenable because they are assumed to implement record- ing systems, not conceptual systems. A perceptual theory of knowledge is developed here in the context of current cognitive science and neuroscience. During perceptual experience, association areas in the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   742 citations  
  • Learning to recognise objects.Guy Wallis & Heinrich Bülthoff - 1999 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3 (1):22-31.
    Evidence from neurophysiological and psychological studies is coming together to shed light on how we represent and recognize objects. This review describes evidence supporting two major hypotheses: the first is that objects are represented in a mosaic-like form in which objects are encoded by combinations of complex, reusable features, rather than two-dimensional templates, or three-dimensional models. The second hypothesis is that transform-invariant representations of objects are learnt through experience, and that this learning is affected by the temporal sequence in which (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Zenon Pylyshyn, "Computation and Cognition: Toward a Foundation for Cognitive Science" and Alvin I. Goldman, "Epistemology and Cognition". [REVIEW]Andy Clark - 1988 - Philosophical Quarterly 38 (153):526-532.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   299 citations  
  • What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action.Arthur M. Glenberg - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (1):1-19.
    I address the commentators' calls for clarification of theoretical terms, discussion of similarities to other proposals, and extension of the ideas. In doing so, I keep the focus on the purpose of memory: enabling the organism to make sense of its environment so that it can take action appropriate to constraints resulting from the physical, personal, social, and cultural situations.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   216 citations  
  • Spatial representations activated during real‐time comprehension of verbs.Daniel C. Richardson, Michael J. Spivey, Lawrence W. Barsalou & Ken McRae - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (5):767-780.
    Previous research has shown that na_ve participants display a high level of agreement when asked to choose or drawschematic representations, or image schemas, of concrete and abstract verbs [Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 2001, Erlbaum, Mawhah, NJ, p. 873]. For example, participants tended to ascribe a horizontal image schema to push, and a vertical image schema to respect. This consistency in offline data is preliminary evidence that language invokes spatial forms of representation. It also (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • Hemispheric differences in semantic-relatedness judgments.Rolf A. Zwaan & Richard H. Yaxley - 2003 - Cognition 87 (3):B79-B86.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Dynamic mental representations.Jennifer J. Freyd - 1987 - Psychological Review 94 (4):427-438.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   86 citations