Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. (1 other version)Connectionist Natural Language Processing: The State of the Art.M. H. Christiansen, N. Chater & M. S. Seidenberg - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):417-437.
    This Special Issue on Connectionist Models of Human Language Processing provides an opportunity for an appraisal both of specific connectionist models and of the status and utility of connectionist models of language in general. This introduction provides the background for the papers in the Special Issue. The development of connectionist models of language is traced, from their intellectual origins, to the state of current research. Key themes that arise throughout different areas of connectionist psycholinguistics are highlighted, and recent developments in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • (1 other version)Connectionist Natural Language Processing: The State of the Art.Morten H. Christiansen & Nick Chater - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):417-437.
    This Special Issue on Connectionist Models of Human Language Processing provides an opportunity for an appraisal both of specific connectionist models and of the status and utility of connectionist models of language in general. This introduction provides the background for the papers in the Special Issue. The development of connectionist models of language is traced, from their intellectual origins, to the state of current research. Key themes that arise throughout different areas of connectionist psycholinguistics are highlighted, and recent developments in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • From rote learning to system building: acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets.Kim Plunkett & Virginia Marchman - 1993 - Cognition 48 (1):21-69.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   58 citations  
  • Dynamisch Inter(-en trans)disciplinair Taal Onderzoek: De nieuwe taalwetenschappen.Nathalie Gontier & Katrien Mondt (eds.) - 2006 - Gent, België: Academia press, Ginkgo.
    Language research is currently in a state of flux. The phenomenon of language is not merely the topic of investigation in linguistics, it is examined by a multitude of scholars with different scientific backgrounds. In order to examine how these various disciplines approach language, a think-tank was founded in 2002, called DITO, Dynamisch Inter(-en trans)disciplinair onderzoek, or Dynamic Inter- (and trans)disciplinary Research. The think-tank is located at the Belgian Vrije Universiteit Brussel (Free University of Brussels). This book provides short introductory (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning from Grammatical SLI: Response to JB Tomblin and J. Pandich (1999).Heather K. J. Van der Lely - 1999 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3 (8):286-288.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Why do children learn to say “Broke”? A model of learning the past tense without feedback.Niels A. Taatgen & John R. Anderson - 2002 - Cognition 86 (2):123-155.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  • Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variables.Joseph Shimron, Iris Berent & Stephen Pinker - 1999 - Cognition 72 (1):1-44.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • (1 other version)Recursion in children's word formation: an examination of “exceptions” to Level Ordering.M. Alegre & Peter Gordon - 1996 - Cognition 60:65-82.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Morphological priming in the German mental lexicon.Ingrid Sonnenstuhl, Sonja Eisenbeiss & Harald Clahsen - 1999 - Cognition 72 (3):203-236.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Developing biases.Ruben van de Vijver & Dinah Baer-Henney - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5:83070.
    German nouns may alternate from singular to plural in two different ways. Some singular forms that end in a voiceless obstruent have a plural in which this obstruent is voiced. Another alternation concerns the vowel. Some singular forms with a back vowel have a plural form in which this back vowel is front. For each noun it has to be established individually whether it alternates or not. The voicing alternation is phonetically grounded, but the vowel alternation is not. Knowledge about (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Lexical word formation in children with grammatical SLI: a grammar-specific versus an input-processing deficit?Heather K. J. van der Lely & Valerie Christian - 2000 - Cognition 75 (1):33-63.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Lexical storage and regular processes.Geert Booij - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6):1016-1016.
    Clahsen's claim that output forms of productive processes are never listed in the lexicon is a consequence of the rule/list fallacy, empirically incorrect, and not necessary for the hypothesis that the human language faculty has a dual structure, that is, a lexicon and a set of rules.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • (1 other version)Red rats eater exposes recursion in children's word formation.Maria A. Alegre & Peter Gordon - 1996 - Cognition 60 (1):65-82.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Errors of Omission in English‐Speaking Children's Production of Plurals and the Past Tense: The Effects of Frequency, Phonology, and Competition.Danielle E. Matthews & Anna L. Theakston - 2006 - Cognitive Science 30 (6):1027-1052.
    How do English‐speaking children inflect nouns for plurality and verbs for the past tense? We assess theoretical answers to this question by considering errors of omission, which occur when children produce a stem in place of its inflected counterpart (e.g., saying “dress” to refer to 5 dresses). A total of 307 children (aged 3;11–9;9) participated in 3 inflection studies. In Study 1, we show that errors of omission occur until the age of 7 and are more likely with both sibilant (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • (1 other version)Children's Frequency , Productivity Phonology, in the and English Past Tense : The Role of Neighborhood Structure.Virginia A. Marchman - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (3):283-304.
    The productive use of English past tense morphology in school-aged children (N= 74; 3 years, 8 months to 13 years, 5 months) is explored using on elicited production task. Errors represented 20% of the responses overall. Virtually all of the children demonstrated productivity with regular (e.g., good) and irregular patterns (zero-marking, e.g., sit + sit; vowel-change, e.g., ride -+ rid). Overall frequency of errors decreased with age, yet the tendency for certain types of irregularizations increased in the older groups. Analyses (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Canonical linking rules: forward versus reverse linking in normally developing and specifically language-impaired children.Heather K. J. van der Lely - 1994 - Cognition 51 (1):29-72.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • (1 other version)Children's Productivity in the English Past Tense: The Role of Frequency, Phonology, and Neighborhood Structure.Virginia A. Marchman - 1997 - Cognitive Science 21 (3):283-304.
    The productive use of English past tense morphology in school‐aged children (N = 74; 3 years, 8 months to 13 years, 5 months) is explored using an elicited production task. Errors represented 20% of the responses overall. Virtually all of the children demonstrated productivity with regular (e.g., good) and irregular patterns (zero‐marking, e.g., sit → sit; vowel‐change, e.g., ride → rid). Overall frequency of errors decreased with age, yet the tendency for certain types of irregularizations increased in the older groups. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations