Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Does Perceptual Simulation Explain Spatial Effects in Word Categorization?Barbara Treccani, Claudio Mulatti, Simone Sulpizio & Remo Job - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Variability in the Alignment of Number and Space Across Languages and Tasks.Andrea Bender, Annelie Rothe-Wulf & Sieghard Beller - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Finger Counting Habits and Spatial-Numerical Association in Horizontal and Vertical Orientations.Marco Fabbri - 2013 - Journal of Cognition and Culture 13 (1-2):95-110.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A Taxonomy Proposal for Types of Interactions of Language and Place-Value Processing in Multi-Digit Numbers.Julia Bahnmueller, Hans-Christoph Nuerk & Korbinian Moeller - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Editorial: The role of reasoning in mathematical thinking.Kinga Morsanyi, Jérôme Prado & Lindsey E. Richland - 2018 - Thinking and Reasoning 24 (2):129-137.
    Research into mathematics often focuses on basic numerical and spatial intuitions, and one key property of numbers: their magnitude. The fact that mathematics is a system of complex relationships that invokes reasoning usually receives less attention. The purpose of this special issue is to highlight the intricate connections between reasoning and mathematics, and to use insights from the reasoning literature to obtain a more complete understanding of the processes that underlie mathematical cognition. The topics that are discussed range from the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Lateralisation of emotions: evidence from pupil size measurement.L. Lichtenstein-Vidne, S. Gabay, N. Cohen & A. Henik - 2017 - Cognition and Emotion 31 (4):699-711.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • (1 other version)Spatial numerical associations in preschoolers.Catherine Thevenot, Michel Fayol & Pierre Barrouillet - 2017 - Thinking and Reasoning 24 (2):221-233.
    Three-to-five-year-old French children were asked to add or remove objects to or from linear displays. The hypothesis of a universal tendency to represent increasing number magnitudes from left to right led to predict a majority of manipulations at the right end of the rows, whatever children's hand laterality. Conversely, if numbers are not inherently associated with space, children were expected to favour laterality-consistent manipulations. The results showed a strong tendency to operate on the right end of the rows in right-handers, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Dealing with Big Numbers: Representation and Understanding of Magnitudes Outside of Human Experience.Resnick Ilyse, S. Newcombe Nora & F. Shipley Thomas - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (4):1020-1041.
    Being able to estimate quantity is important in everyday life and for success in the STEM disciplines. However, people have difficulty reasoning about magnitudes outside of human perception. This study examines patterns of estimation errors across temporal and spatial magnitudes at large scales. We evaluated the effectiveness of hierarchical alignment in improving estimations, and transfer across dimensions. The activity was successful in increasing accuracy for temporal and spatial magnitudes, and learning transferred to the estimation of numeric magnitudes associated with events (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Innate and Cultural Spatial Time: A Developmental Perspective.Barbara Magnani & Alessandro Musetti - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
    We reviewed literature to understand when a spatial map for time is available in the brain. We carefully defined the concepts of metrical map of time and of conceptual representation of time as the mental time line (MTL) in order to formulate our position. It is that both metrical map and conceptual representation of time are spatial in nature. The former should be innate, related to motor/implicit timing, it should represent all magnitudes with an analogic and bi-dimensional structure. The latter (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The emergence of temporal language in Nicaraguan Sign Language.Annemarie Kocab, Ann Senghas & Jesse Snedeker - 2016 - Cognition 156 (C):147-163.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The color red attracts attention in an emotional context. An ERP study.Michał Kuniecki, Joanna Pilarczyk & Szymon Wichary - 2015 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Polarity correspondence effect between loudness and lateralized response set.Seah Chang & Yang Seok Cho - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Arbitrary numbers counter fair decisions: trails of markedness in card distribution.Philipp A. Schroeder & Roland Pfister - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Sequential modulation of distractor-interference produced by semantic generalization of stimulus features.Mike Wendt, Aquiles Luna-Rodriguez & Thomas Jacobsen - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Embodied Language Comprehension Requires an Enactivist Paradigm of Cognition.Michiel van Elk, Marc Slors & Harold Bekkering - 2010 - Frontiers in Psychology 1.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • A helping hand putting in order: Visuomotor routines organize numerical and non-numerical sequences in space.Luca Rinaldi, Samuel Di Luca, Avishai Henik & Luisa Girelli - 2016 - Cognition 152 (C):40-52.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Space and time in the sighted and blind.Roberto Bottini, Davide Crepaldi, Daniel Casasanto, Virgine Crollen & Olivier Collignon - 2015 - Cognition 141 (C):67-72.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Algebraic Models of Mental Number Axes: Part II.Wojciech Krysztofiak - 2016 - Axiomathes 26 (2):123-155.
    The paper presents a formal model of the system of number representations as a multiplicity of mental number axes with a hierarchical structure. The hierarchy is determined by the mind as it acquires successive types of mental number axes generated by virtue of some algebraic mechanisms. Three types of algebraic structures, responsible for functioning these mechanisms, are distinguished: BASAN-structures, CASAN-structures and CAPPAN-structures. A foundational order holds between these structures. CAPPAN-structures are derivative from CASAN-structures which are extensions of BASAN-structures. The constructed (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Are Automatic Conceptual Cores the Gold Standard of Semantic Processing? The Context‐Dependence of Spatial Meaning in Grounded Congruency Effects.Lauren A. M. Lebois, Christine D. Wilson-Mendenhall & Lawrence W. Barsalou - 2015 - Cognitive Science 39 (8):1764-1801.
    According to grounded cognition, words whose semantics contain sensory-motor features activate sensory-motor simulations, which, in turn, interact with spatial responses to produce grounded congruency effects. Growing evidence shows these congruency effects do not always occur, suggesting instead that the grounded features in a word's meaning do not become active automatically across contexts. Researchers sometimes use this as evidence that concepts are not grounded, further concluding that grounded information is peripheral to the amodal cores of concepts. We first review broad evidence (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • Modulation of motor-meaning congruity effects for valenced words.G. Brookshire, Daniel Casasanto & Richard Ivry - 2010 - In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 1940--1945.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Left–right coding of past and future in language: The mental timeline during sentence processing.Rolf Ulrich & Claudia Maienborn - 2010 - Cognition 117 (2):126-138.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Do monkeys think in metaphors? Representations of space and time in monkeys and humans.Dustin J. Merritt, Daniel Casasanto & Elizabeth M. Brannon - 2010 - Cognition 117 (2):191-202.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Mental movements without magnitude? A study of spatial biases in symbolic arithmetic.Michal Pinhas & Martin H. Fischer - 2008 - Cognition 109 (3):408-415.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • Nature and culture of finger counting: Diversity and representational effects of an embodied cognitive tool.Andrea Bender & Sieghard Beller - 2012 - Cognition 124 (2):156-182.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Representation of numerical and non-numerical order in children.Ilaria Berteletti, Daniela Lucangeli & Marco Zorzi - 2012 - Cognition 124 (3):304-313.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Changing auditory time with prismatic goggles.Barbara Magnani, Francesco Pavani & Francesca Frassinetti - 2012 - Cognition 125 (2):233-243.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • How decisions emerge: Action dynamics in intertemporal decision making.Maja Dshemuchadse, Stefan Scherbaum & Thomas Goschke - 2013 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 142 (1):93.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Single-digit and two-digit Arabic numerals address the same semantic number line.Bert Reynvoet & Marc Brysbaert - 1999 - Cognition 72 (2):191-201.
    Many theories about human number representation stress the importance of a central semantic representation that includes the magnitude information of small integer numbers, and that is conceived as an abstract, compressed number line. However, thus far there has been little or no direct evidence that units and teens are represented on the same number line. In two masked priming experiments, we show that single-digit and two-digit Arabic numerals are equally well primed by an Arabic numeral with the same number of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • How do we convert a number into a finger trajectory?Dror Dotan & Stanislas Dehaene - 2013 - Cognition 129 (3):512-529.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Semantic sides of three-dimensional space representation.Arnaud Badets - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (5):543 - 543.
    In this commentary, I propose that horizontal and vertical dimensions of space are represented together inside a common metrics mechanism located in the parietal cortex. Importantly, this network is also involved in the processing of number magnitudes and environment-directed actions. Altogether, the evidence suggests that different magnitude dimensions could be intertwined with the horizontality and verticality of our world representation.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Singing Numbers… in Cognitive Space — A Dual‐Task Study of the Link Between Pitch, Space, and Numbers.Martin H. Fischer, Marianna Riello, Bruno L. Giordano & Elena Rusconi - 2013 - Topics in Cognitive Science 5 (2):354-366.
    We assessed the automaticity of spatial-numerical and spatial-musical associations by testing their intentionality and load sensitivity in a dual-task paradigm. In separate sessions, 16 healthy adults performed magnitude and pitch comparisons on sung numbers with variable pitch. Stimuli and response alternatives were identical, but the relevant stimulus attribute (pitch or number) differed between tasks. Concomitant tasks required retention of either color or location information. Results show that spatial associations of both magnitude and pitch are load sensitive and that the spatial (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Time–space synaesthesia – A cognitive advantage?Heather Mann, Jason Korzenko, Jonathan S. A. Carriere & Mike J. Dixon - 2009 - Consciousness and Cognition 18 (3):619-627.
    Is synaesthesia cognitively useful? Individuals with time–space synaesthesia experience time units as idiosyncratic spatial forms, and report that these forms aid them in mentally organising their time. In the present study, we hypothesised that time–space synaesthesia would facilitate performance on a time-related cognitive task. Synaesthetes were not specifically recruited for participation; instead, likelihood of time–space synaesthesia was assessed on a continuous scale based on participants’ responses during a semi-structured interview. Participants performed a month-manipulation task, which involved naming every second month (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Don't throw the baby out with the math water: Why discounting the developmental foundations of early numeracy is premature and unnecessary.Kevin Muldoon, Charlie Lewis & Norman Freeman - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (6):663-664.
    We see no grounds for insisting that, because the concept natural number is abstract, its foundations must be innate. It is possible to specify domain general learning processes that feed into more abstract concepts of numerical infinity. By neglecting the messiness of children's slow acquisition of arithmetical concepts, Rips et al. present an idealized, unnecessarily insular, view of number development.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • From numerical concepts to concepts of number.Lance J. Rips, Amber Bloomfield & Jennifer Asmuth - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (6):623-642.
    Many experiments with infants suggest that they possess quantitative abilities, and many experimentalists believe that these abilities set the stage for later mathematics: natural numbers and arithmetic. However, the connection between these early and later skills is far from obvious. We evaluate two possible routes to mathematics and argue that neither is sufficient: (1) We first sketch what we think is the most likely model for infant abilities in this domain, and we examine proposals for extrapolating the natural number concept (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Symbolic and nonsymbolic pathways of number processing.Tom Verguts & Wim Fias - 2008 - Philosophical Psychology 21 (4):539 – 554.
    Recent years have witnessed an enormous increase in behavioral and neuroimaging studies of numerical cognition. Particular interest has been devoted toward unraveling properties of the representational medium on which numbers are thought to be represented. We have argued that a correct inference concerning these properties requires distinguishing between different input modalities and different decision/output structures. To back up this claim, we have trained computational models with either symbolic or nonsymbolic input and with different task requirements, and showed that this allowed (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Author's response: Is number sense a patchwork?Stanislas Dehaene - 2001 - Mind and Language 16 (1):89–100.
    ‘Number sense’ is a short‐hand for our ability to quickly understand, approximate, and manipulate numerical quantities. My hypothesis is that number sense rests on cerebral circuits that have evolved specifically for the purpose of representing basic arithmetic knowledge. Four lines of evidence suggesting that number sense constitutes a domain‐specific, biologically‐determined ability are reviewed: the presence of evolutionary precursors of arithmetic in animals; the early emergence of arithmetic competence in infants independently of other abilities, including language; the existence of a homology (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Unconscious priming according to multiple s-r rules.Andrea Kiesel, Wilfried Kunde & Joachim Hoffmann - 2007 - Cognition 104 (1):89-105.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Unconscious semantic priming extends to novel unseen stimuli.Lionel Naccache & Stanislas Dehaene - 2001 - Cognition 80 (3):215-229.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   66 citations  
  • Core knowledge as a neuro-ethologist views it.Giorgio Vallortigara - 2024 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 47:e144.
    Innateness of core knowledge mechanisms (in the form of “cognitive priors”) can be revealed by proper comparisons of altricial and precocial species. Cognitive priors and sensitive periods in their expression may also provide clues for the development of plausible artificial intelligence systems.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Don't SNARC me now! Intraindividual variability of cognitive phenomena – Insights from the Ironman paradigm.Lilly Roth, Verena Jordan, Stefania Schwarz, Klaus Willmes, Hans-Christoph Nuerk, Jean-Philippe van Dijck & Krzysztof Cipora - 2024 - Cognition 248 (C):105781.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Why fractions are difficult? Modeling optimal and sub-optimal integration strategies of numerators and denominators by educated adults.Daniel Fitousi & Ran Noyman - 2024 - Cognition 242 (C):105656.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Metacognition in working memory: Confidence judgments during an n-back task.Nadia Conte, Beth Fairfield, Caterina Padulo & Santiago Pelegrina - 2023 - Consciousness and Cognition 111 (C):103522.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The link between transitive reasoning and mathematics achievement in preadolescence: the role of relational processing and deductive reasoning.Terry Tin-Yau Wong & Kinga Morsanyi - 2023 - Thinking and Reasoning 29 (4):531-558.
    The link between logic and mathematics has long been recognized by theorists from various fields. For instance, the mathematician, Bertrand Russell (1919), described logic and math as intrinsically...
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Effects of attentional shifts along the vertical axis on number processing: An eye-tracking study with optokinetic stimulation.Arianna Felisatti, Mariagrazia Ranzini, Elvio Blini, Matteo Lisi & Marco Zorzi - 2022 - Cognition 221 (C):104991.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How Cognitive Strengths Compensate Weaknesses Related to Specific Learning Difficulties in Fourth-Grade Children.Marije D. E. Huijsmans, Tijs Kleemans & Evelyn H. Kroesbergen - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12:552458.
    The goal of the present study was to investigate whether children’s cognitive strengths can compensate the accompanied weaknesses related to their specific learning difficulties. A Bayesian multigroup mediation SEM analysis in 281 fourth-grade children identified a cognitive compensatory mechanism in children with mathematical learning difficulties (n= 36): Children with weak number sense, but strong rapid naming performed slightly better on mathematics compared to peers with weak rapid naming. In contrast, a compensatory mechanism was not identified for children with a comorbid (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does Number Perception Cause Automatic Shifts of Spatial Attention? A Study of the Att-SNARC Effect in Numbers and Chinese Months.Dexian He, Xianyou He, Tingting Zhao, Jing Wang, Longzhao Li & Max Louwerse - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A Mental Odd-Even Continuum Account: Some Numbers May Be “More Odd” Than Others and Some Numbers May Be “More Even” Than Others.Lia Heubner, Krzysztof Cipora, Mojtaba Soltanlou, Marie-Lene Schlenker, Katarzyna Lipowska, Silke M. Göbel, Frank Domahs, Maciej Haman & Hans-Christoph Nuerk - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:364587.
    Numerical categories such as parity, i.e., being odd or even, have frequently been shown to influence how particular numbers are processed. Mathematically, number parity is defined categorically. So far, cognitive, and psychological accounts have followed the mathematical definition and defined parity as a categorical psychological representation as well. In this manuscript, we wish to test the alternative account that cognitively, parity is represented in a more gradual manner such that some numbers are represented as “more odd” or “more even” than (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Spatial-Numerical Associations Enhance the Short-Term Memorization of Digit Locations.Catherine Thevenot, Jasinta Dewi, Pamela B. Lavenex & Jeanne Bagnoud - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The mechanism of non-numerical anchoring heuristic based on magnitude priming: is it just the basic anchoring effect in disguise?Jakub Traczyk & Pawel Tomczak - 2017 - Polish Psychological Bulletin 48 (3):401-410.
    The anchoring heuristic refers to phenomena when an arbitrary number affects subsequent numerical estimations. Oppenheimer, LeBoeuf and Brewer showed that it is not necessary for the anchor to be a numerical value, yet current models describing the anchoring heuristic do not fully account for the mechanism of non-numerical anchoring. However, this effect shows similarity to the basic anchoring effect - obtained without the comparative question and based on the availability of the given number in working memory. In this study, we (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Commentary: The poverty of embodied cognition.Kinga Wołoszyn & Mateusz Hohol - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations