Switch to: References

Citations of:

Attention and Effort

Prentice-Hall (1973)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Apertures, Draw, and Syntax: Remodeling Attention.Brian Bruya - 2010 - In Effortless Attention: A New Perspective in the Cognitive Science of Attention and Action. MIT Press. pp. 219.
    Because psychological studies of attention and cognition are most commonly performed within the strict confines of the laboratory or take cognitively impaired patients as subjects, it is difficult to be sure that resultant models of attention adequately account for the phenomenon of effortless attention. The problem is not only that effortless attention is resistant to laboratory study. A further issue is that because the laboratory is the most common way to approach attention, models resulting from such studies are naturally the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Daniel Kahneman: the Nobel Prize for Economics awarded for Decision-making psychology.Rino Rumiati & Nicolao Bonini - 2002 - Mind and Society 3 (1):VII-XI.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The rehabilitation of spontaneity: A new approach in philosophy of action.Brian J. Bruya - 2010 - Philosophy East and West 60 (2):pp. 207-250.
    Scholars working in philosophy of action still struggle with the freedom/determinism dichotomy that stretches back to Hellenist philosophy and the metaphysics that gave rise to it. Although that metaphysics has been repudiated in current philosophy of mind and cognitive science, the dichotomy still haunts these fields. As such, action is understood as distinct from movement, or motion. In early China, under a very different metaphysical paradigm, no such distinction is made. Instead, a notion of self-caused movement, or spontaneity, is elaborated. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Theory knitting: An integrative approach to theory development.David A. Kalmar & Robert J. Sternberg - 1988 - Philosophical Psychology 1 (2):153 – 170.
    A close scrutiny of the psychological literature reveals that many psychologists favor a 'segregative' approach to theory development. One theory is pitted against another, and the one that accounts for the data most successfully is deemed the theory of choice. However, an examination of the theoretical debates in which the segregative approach has been pursued reveals a variety of weaknesses to the approach, namely, masking an underlying theoretical indistinguishability of theoretical predictions, causing psychologists to focus unknowingly on different aspects of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Codes and their vicissitudes.Bernhard Hommel, Jochen Müsseler, Gisa Aschersleben & Wolfgang Prinz - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (5):910-926.
    First, we discuss issues raised with respect to the Theory of Event Coding (TEC)'s scope, that is, its limitations and possible extensions. Then, we address the issue of specificity, that is, the widespread concern that TEC is too unspecified and, therefore, too vague in a number of important respects. Finally, we elaborate on our views about TEC's relations to other important frameworks and approaches in the field like stages models, ecological approaches, and the two-visual-pathways model. Footnotes1 We acknowledge the precedence (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology.Graeme S. Halford, William H. Wilson & Steven Phillips - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (6):803-831.
    Working memory limits are best defined in terms of the complexity of the relations that can be processed in parallel. Complexity is defined as the number of related dimensions or sources of variation. A unary relation has one argument and one source of variation; its argument can be instantiated in only one way at a time. A binary relation has two arguments, two sources of variation, and two instantiations, and so on. Dimensionality is related to the number of chunks, because (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  • Consciousness from a first-person perspective.Max Velmans - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):702-726.
    This paper replies to the first 36 commentaries on my target article on “Is human information processing conscious?” (Behavioral and Brain Sciences,1991, pp.651-669). The target article focused largely on experimental studies of how consciousness relates to human information processing, tracing their relation from input through to output, while discussion of the implications of the findings both for cognitive psychology and philosophy of mind was relatively brief. The commentaries reversed this emphasis, and so, correspondingly, did the reply. The sequence of topics (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   132 citations  
  • What is self-control?Edmund Henden - 2008 - Philosophical Psychology 21 (1):69 – 90.
    What is self-control and how does the concept of self-control relate to the notion of will-power? A widespread philosophical opinion has been that the notion of will-power does not add anything beyond what can be said using other motivational notions, such as strength of desire and intention. One exception is Richard Holton who, inspired by recent research in social psychology, has argued that will-power is a separate faculty needed for persisting in one's resolutions, what he calls 'strength of will'. However, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Economics and Hermeneutics.Lawrence A. Berger - 1989 - Economics and Philosophy 5 (2):209-234.
    In a recent article in this journal, D. Wade Hands reviewed Charles Taylor's two-volume work, Philosophical Papers. Hands predicts that Taylor's work will have no impact on the philosophy of economics. This may indeed turn out to be the case; but if so, it will only be because the profession is not listening. Of course, it is typical of the profession to be more interested in exporting its product than in learning from other disciplines. This is exemplified in Hands's use (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal.Daniel Holender - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):1-23.
    When the stored representation of the meaning of a stimulus is accessed through the processing of a sensory input it is maintained in an activated state for a certain amount of time that allows for further processing. This semantic activation is generally accompanied by conscious identification, which can be demonstrated by the ability of a person to perform discriminations on the basis of the meaning of the stimulus. The idea that a sensory input can give rise to semantic activation without (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   447 citations  
  • Sensory awareness versus sensory impression: Affect and attention interact to produce somatic meaning.Delia Cioffi - 1991 - Cognition and Emotion 5 (4):275-94.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Controlled and automatic human information processing: Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory.Richard M. Shiffrin & Walter Schneider - 1977 - Psychological Review 84 (2):128-90.
    Tested the 2-process theory of detection, search, and attention presented by the current authors in a series of experiments. The studies demonstrate the qualitative difference between 2 modes of information processing: automatic detection and controlled search; trace the course of the learning of automatic detection, of categories, and of automatic-attention responses; and show the dependence of automatic detection on attending responses and demonstrate how such responses interrupt controlled processing and interfere with the focusing of attention. The learning of categories is (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   800 citations  
  • Depth psychology and self-deception.Robert Lockie - 2003 - Philosophical Psychology 16 (1):127-148.
    This paper argues that self-deception cannot be explained without employing a depth-psychological ("psychodynamic") notion of the unconscious, and therefore that mainstream academic psychology must make space for such approaches. The paper begins by explicating the notion of a dynamic unconscious. Then a brief account is given of the "paradoxes" of self-deception. It is shown that a depth-psychological self of parts and subceptive agency removes any such paradoxes. Next, several competing accounts of self-deception are considered: an attentional account, a constructivist account, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Is human information processing conscious?Max Velmans - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):651-69.
    Investigations of the function of consciousness in human information processing have focused mainly on two questions: (1) where does consciousness enter into the information processing sequence and (2) how does conscious processing differ from preconscious and unconscious processing. Input analysis is thought to be initially "preconscious," "pre-attentive," fast, involuntary, and automatic. This is followed by "conscious," "focal-attentive" analysis which is relatively slow, voluntary, and flexible. It is thought that simple, familiar stimuli can be identified preconsciously, but conscious processing is needed (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   348 citations  
  • Size Perception of a Sport Target as a Function of Practice Success Conditions.Krystina Bianchi, Molly Brillinger & Jae Todd Patterson - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Superior motor task success has been correlated with participants’ self-reports of a larger-than-actual size of a sport-related target. In the present study, we examined whether a putting practice condition with greater success would differentially impact participants’ self-reported perceptions of the size of the putting hole during acquisition and retention. We randomly assigned participants to one of three different practice conditions and had them self-report their perceived size of the putting hole upon completion of each required putting distance. Although there were (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mental Effort When Playing, Listening, and Imagining Music in One Pianist’s Eyes and Brain.Tor Endestad, Rolf Inge Godøy, Markus Handal Sneve, Thomas Hagen, Agata Bochynska & Bruno Laeng - 2020 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 14.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive Resources Necessary for Motor Control in Older Adults Are Reduced by Walking and Coordination Training.Ben Godde & Claudia Voelcker-Rehage - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Immersive Virtual Reality as an Adjunctive Non-opioid Analgesic for Pre-dominantly Latin American Children With Large Severe Burn Wounds During Burn Wound Cleaning in the Intensive Care Unit: A Pilot Study.Hunter G. Hoffman, Robert A. Rodriguez, Miriam Gonzalez, Mary Bernardy, Raquel Peña, Wanda Beck, David R. Patterson & Walter J. Meyer - 2019 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Influences of Postural Control on Cognitive Control in Task Switching.Denise N. Stephan, Sandra Hensen, Edina Fintor, Ralf Krampe & Iring Koch - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Effect of Passive Hyperthermia on Working Memory Resources during Simple and Complex Cognitive Tasks.Nadia Gaoua, Christopher P. Herrera, Julien D. Périard, Farid El Massioui & Sebastien Racinais - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Implicit and Explicit Knowledge Both Improve Dual Task Performance in a Continuous Pursuit Tracking Task.Harald E. Ewolds, Laura Bröker, Rita F. de Oliveira, Markus Raab & Stefan Künzell - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mechanisms of Mind-Body Interaction and Optimal Performance.Yi-Yuan Tang & Brian Bruya - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Based on recent findings, we propose a framework for a relationship among attention, effort and optimal performance. Optimal performance often refers to an effortless and automatic, flow-like state of performance. Mindfulness regulates the focus of attention to optimal focus on the core component of the action, avoiding too much attention that could be detrimental for elite performance. Balanced attention is a trained state that can optimize any particular attentional activity on the dual-process spectrum.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Challenge-driven attention: Interacting frontal and brainstem systems.Rajeev D. S. Raizada - 2008 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 1.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The bright side of being blue: Depression as an adaptation for analyzing complex problems.Paul W. Andrews & J. Anderson Thomson - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (3):620-654.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations  
  • An integrated theory of attention and decision making in visual signal detection.Philip L. Smith & Roger Ratcliff - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (2):283-317.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Cognitive aging and hearing acuity: modeling spoken language comprehension.Arthur Wingfield, Nicole M. Amichetti & Amanda Lash - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Mental workload and driving.Julie Paxion, Edith Galy & Catherine Berthelon - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5:88843.
    The aim of this chapter is to identify the most representative measures of subjective and objective mental workload in driving, and to understand how the subjective and objective levels of mental workload influence the performance as a function of situation complexity and driving experience, i.e. to verify whether the increase of situation complexity and the lack of experience increase the subjective and physiological levels of mental workload and lead to driving performance impairments. This chapter will be useful to carry out (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The neurocognitive consequences of the wandering mind: a mechanistic account of sensory-motor decoupling.Julia W. Y. Kam & Todd C. Handy - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Competition explains limited attention and perceptual resources: implications for perceptual load and dilution theories.Paige E. Scalf, Ana Torralbo, Evelina Tapia & Diane M. Beck - 2013 - Frontiers in Psychology 4.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Attention demands of spoken word planning: a review.Ardi Roelofs - 2011 - Frontiers in Psychology 2.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Fast and frugal versus regression models of human judgement.Mandeep K. Dhami Clare Harries - 2001 - Thinking and Reasoning 7 (1):5-27.
    Following Brunswik (1952), social judgement theorists have long relied on regression models to describe both an individual's judgements and the environment about which such judgements are made. However, social judgement theory is not synonymous with these compensatory, static, structural models. We compared the characterisations of physicians' judgements using a regression model with that of a non-compensatory process model (called fast and frugal). We found that both models fit the data equally well. Both models suggest that physicians use few cues, that (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Mental timing and the central attentional bottleneck.Eric Ruthruff & Harold Pashler - 2010 - In Anna C. Nobre & Jennifer T. Coull (eds.), Attention and Time. Oxford University Press. pp. 123--135.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Attentional theories and conscious perception.Benjamin Libet - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):247-248.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Attentional influence on the mismatch negativity.Marty G. Woldorff & Steven A. Hillyard - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):258-260.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Consciousness may still have a processing role to play.Robert Van Gulick - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):699-700.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   63 citations  
  • Consciousness and content in learning: Missing or misconceived?Richard A. Carlson - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):673-674.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Lexical access and discourse planning: Bottom-up interference or top-down control troubles?Wendy G. Lehnert - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):528-529.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • When is an image hallucinatory?Graham F. Reed - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):530-531.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Image or neural coding of inner speech and agency?Gail Zivin - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):534-535.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Verbal hallucinations also occur in normals.Thomas B. Posey - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (3):530-530.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Why parapsychology cannot become a science.Mario Bunge - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):576.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The evolution of science and “principles of impossibility”.Victor G. Adamenko - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):566.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A cognitive scientist's view of intelligence.Allan Collins - 1980 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3 (4):588-589.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Anomaly versus artifact, or anomalous artifact?Marcello Truzzi - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (4):614.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The relationship between naturally occurring dysphoric moods, elaborative encoding, and recall performance.Richard Potts, Cameron Camp & Cheryl Coyne - 1989 - Cognition and Emotion 3 (3):197-205.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance.Robert Kurzban, Angela Duckworth, Joseph W. Kable & Justus Myers - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (6):661-679.
    Why does performing certain tasks cause the aversive experience of mental effort and concomitant deterioration in task performance? One explanation posits a physical resource that is depleted over time. We propose an alternative explanation that centers on mental representations of the costs and benefits associated with task performance. Specifically, certain computational mechanisms, especially those associated with executive function, can be deployed for only a limited number of simultaneous tasks at any given moment. Consequently, the deployment of these computational mechanisms carries (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   100 citations  
  • The subject of attention.Carolyn Dicey Jennings - 2012 - Synthese 189 (3):535-554.
    The absence of a common understanding of attention plagues current research on the topic. Combining the findings from three domains of research on attention, this paper presents a univocal account that fits normal use of the term as well as its many associated phenomena: attention is a process of mental selection that is within the control of the subject. The role of the subject is often excluded from naturalized accounts, but this paper will be an exception to that rule. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • The what and why of binding: The modeler's perspective.Christoph von der Malsburg - 1999 - Neuron 24:95-104.
    In attempts to formulate a computational understanding of brain function, one of the fundamental concerns is the data structure by which the brain represents information. For many decades, a conceptual framework has dominated the thinking of both brain modelers and neurobiologists. That framework is referred to here as "classical neural networks." It is well supported by experimental data, although it may be incomplete. A characterization of this framework will be offered in the next section. Difficulties in modeling important functional aspects (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • Correlates of Silence: Enhanced Microstructural Changes in the Uncinate Fasciculus.Tal Dotan Ben-Soussan, Fabio Marson, Claudia Piervincenzi, Joseph Glicksohn, Antonio De Fano, Francesca Amenduni, Carlo C. Quattrocchi & Filippo Carducci - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Variation in Event-Related Potentials by State Transitions.Hiroshi Higashi, Tetsuto Minami & Shigeki Nakauchi - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation