- The early emergence and puzzling decline of relational reasoning: Effects of knowledge and search on inferring abstract concepts.Caren M. Walker, Sophie Bridgers & Alison Gopnik - 2016 - Cognition 156 (C):30-40.details
|
|
The Oxford Handbook of Causal Reasoning.Michael Waldmann (ed.) - 2017 - Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.details
|
|
Interlinking physical beliefs: Children’s bias towards logical congruence.Heidi Kloos - 2007 - Cognition 103 (2):227-252.details
|
|
Initial knowledge: six suggestions.Elizabeth Spelke - 1994 - Cognition 50 (1-3):431-445.details
|
|
On preserving and extending Piaget's contributions.Howard Gardner - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):141-141.details
|
|
Disenshrining the Cartesian self.Barbara A. C. Saunders - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):77-78.details
|
|
Three inferential temptations.Alexander Levine & Georg Schwarz - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):57-58.details
|
|
Analytic functionalism without representational functionalism.Terence Horgan - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):51-51.details
|
|
Competing accounts of belief-task performance.Alvin I. Goldman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):43-44.details
|
|
Recall or regeneration of past mental states: Toward an account in terms of cognitive processes.K. Anders Ericsson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):41-42.details
|
|
How we know our minds: The illusion of first-person knowledge of intentionality.Alison Gopnik - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):1-14.details
|
|
Where's the person?Michael Tomasello - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):84-85.details
|
|
Dual Space Search During Scientific Reasoning.David Klahr & Kevin Dunbar - 1988 - Cognitive Science 12 (1):1-48.details
|
|
A model theory of induction.Philip N. Johnson‐Laird - 1994 - International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 8 (1):5 – 29.details
|
|
Intentionality, mind and folk psychology.Winand H. Dittrich & Stephen E. G. Lea - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):39-41.details
|
|
Why the Child’s Theory of Mind Really Is a Theory.Alison Gopnik & Henry M. Wellman - 1992 - Mind and Language 7 (1-2):145-71.details
|
|
(1 other version)Children’s reasoning about the efficiency of others’ actions: The development of rational action prediction.Gökhan Gönül & Markus Paulus - 2021 - Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 204 (105035).details
|
|
Prompting Children’s Belief Revision About Balance Through Primary and Secondary Sources of Evidence.Nicole E. Larsen, Vaunam P. Venkadasalam & Patricia A. Ganea - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11:541958.details
|
|
(1 other version)Discovering Psychological Principles by Mining Naturally Occurring Data Sets.Robert L. Goldstone & Gary Lupyan - 2016 - Topics in Cognitive Science 8 (3):548-568.details
|
|
Augmenting Instructional Animations with a Body Analogy to Help Children Learn about Physical Systems.Wim T. J. L. Pouw, Tamara van Gog, Rolf A. Zwaan & Fred Paas - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.details
|
|
Equilibration – the central concept of Piaget's theory.Jeanette McCarthy Gallagher - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):141-141.details
|
|
The origins of inquiry: inductive inference and exploration in early childhood.Laura Schulz - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (7):382-389.details
|
|
Language Acquisition Meets Language Evolution.Nick Chater & Morten H. Christiansen - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (7):1131-1157.details
|
|
The scientist as child.Alison Gopnik - 1996 - Philosophy of Science 63 (4):485-514.details
|
|
(1 other version)The psychology of folk psychology.Alvin I. Goldman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):15-28.details
|
|
Minimally counterintuitive stimuli trigger greater curiosity than merely improbable stimuli.Casey Lewry, Sera Gorucu, Emily G. Liquin & Tania Lombrozo - 2023 - Cognition 230 (C):105286.details
|
|
Telling Apart Motor Noise and Exploratory Behavior, in Early Development.Teodora Gliga - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9:344150.details
|
|
Radical constructivism and its failings: Anti‐realism and individualism.Mark Olssen - 1996 - British Journal of Educational Studies 44 (3):275-295.details
|
|
From cathode rays to alpha particles to quantum of action: A rational reconstruction of structure of the atom and its implications for chemistry textbooks.Mansoor Niaz - 1998 - Science Education 82 (5):527-552.details
|
|
Going beyond the evidence: Abstract laws and preschoolers’ responses to anomalous data.Laura E. Schulz, Noah D. Goodman, Joshua B. Tenenbaum & Adrianna C. Jenkins - 2008 - Cognition 109 (2):211-223.details
|
|
Brainerd versus Aristotle with Piaget looking on.Elizabeth Bates - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):138-139.details
|
|
Further replies on invariant sequences, explanation, and other stage criteria.Charles J. Brainerd - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):149-154.details
|
|
Task Structure Versus Cognitive Structure.Robert H. Pollack - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):147-148.details
|
|
Special access lies down with theory-theory.Sydney Shoemaker - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):78-79.details
|
|
Knowing children's minds.Michael Siegal - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):79-80.details
|
|
Why presume analyses are on-line?Georges Rey - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):74-75.details
|
|
Theory-theory theory.Howard Rachlin - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):72-73.details
|
|
Matching and mental-state ascription.Ian Pratt - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):71-72.details
|
|
Knowledge of the psychological states of self and others is not only theory-laden but also data-driven.Chris Moore & John Barresi - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):61-62.details
|
|
The fallibility of first-person knowledge of intentionality.Peter Ludlow & Norah Martin - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):60-60.details
|
|
“Good developmental sequence” and the paradoxes of children's skills.Brian D. Josephson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):53-54.details
|
|
Qualia for propositional attitudes?Frank Jackson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):52-52.details
|
|
Theories and qualities.Alison Gopnik - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):44-45.details
|
|
Goldman has not defeated folk functionalism.James H. Fetzer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):42-43.details
|
|
The developmental history of an illusion.Keith E. Stanovich - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (1):80-81.details
|
|
(1 other version)First Principles Organize Attention to and Learning About Relevant Data: Number and the Animate‐Inanimate Distinction as Examples.Rochel Gelman - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (1):79-106.details
|
|
Looking for'Constraints'in Infants'Perceptual-Cognitive Development.Julie C. Rutkowska - 1991 - Mind and Language 6 (3):215-238.details
|
|
Piagetian epistemology: Equilibration and the teaching of science.Jack A. Rowell - 1989 - Synthese 80 (1):141 - 162.details
|
|
Performance of Motor Sequences in Children at Heightened vs. Low Risk for ASD: A Longitudinal Study from 18 to 36 Months of Age. [REVIEW]Valentina Focaroli, Fabrizio Taffoni, Shelby M. Parsons, Flavio Keller & Jana M. Iverson - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.details
|
|
When the ordinary seems unexpected: evidence for incremental physical knowledge in young infants.Yuyan Luo & Renée Baillargeon - 2005 - Cognition 95 (3):297-328.details
|
|