Switch to: References

Citations of:

Embodiments of Mind

MIT Press (1963)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. (1 other version)Functionalism, computationalism, and mental contents.Gualtiero Piccinini - 2004 - Canadian Journal of Philosophy 34 (3):375-410.
    Some philosophers have conflated functionalism and computationalism. I reconstruct how this came about and uncover two assumptions that made the conflation possible. They are the assumptions that (i) psychological functional analyses are computational descriptions and (ii) everything may be described as performing computations. I argue that, if we want to improve our understanding of both the metaphysics of mental states and the functional relations between them, we should reject these assumptions. # 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • C. S. Peirce and Intersemiotic Translation.Joao Queiroz & Daniella Aguiar - 2015 - In Peter Pericles Trifonas (ed.), International Handbook of Semiotics. Dordrecht: Springer. pp. 201-215.
    Intersemiotic translation (IT) was defined by Roman Jakobson (The Translation Studies Reader, Routledge, London, p. 114, 2000) as “transmutation of signs”—“an interpretation of verbal signs by means of signs of nonverbal sign systems.” Despite its theoretical relevance, and in spite of the frequency in which it is practiced, the phenomenon remains virtually unexplored in terms of conceptual modeling, especially from a semiotic perspective. Our approach is based on two premises: (i) IT is fundamentally a semiotic operation process (semiosis) and (ii) (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The Civilization at a Crossroads: Constructing the Paradigm Shift.Gennady Shkliarevsky - 2017 - Raleigh, NC: Glasstree Publishing.
    The book addresses the broad issue of sustainability of our civilization and seeks to contribute to the ongoing discussion of what many see as its systemic crisis. There is a broad agreement that new creative ideas, initiatives, and solutions are essential for dealing with the current problems. However, despite this recognition, we still know very little about the process of creation and how it works. As a result, our civilization fails to harness the enormous creative potential of humanity. This failure, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How human is SOAR?Roger W. Remington, Michael G. Shafto & Colleen M. Seifert - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):455-455.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pipelines, processing models, and the mindbody problem.John G. Seamon - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):81-82.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Immersive ideals / critical distances : study of the affinity between artistic ideologies in virtual Reality and previous immersive idioms.Joseph Nechvatal (ed.) - 2010 - Berlin: LAP Lambert Academic Publishing AG & Co KG.
    My research into Virtual Reality technology and its central property of immersion has indicated that immersion in Virtual Reality (VR) electronic systems is a significant key to the understanding of contemporary culture as well as considerable aspects of previous culture as detected in the histories of philosophy and the visual arts. The fundamental change in aesthetic perception engendered by immersion, a perception which is connected to the ideal of total-immersion in virtual space, identifies certain shifts in ontology which are relevant (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuro-cybernetics of socio-scientific systems.Masudul Alam Choudhury & Mohammad Shahadat Hossain - 2010 - Mind and Society 9 (1):59-83.
    The field of information technology is broadened up to the domain of ‘learning’ systems and cybernetics. In covering this extension of the field due recourse is made to the epistemological basis of theory construction. When so comprehended, information technology becomes a philosophical inquiry on a variety of social, scientific and technological issues. A new idea that we refer to as neuro-cybernetics is born. The term neuro-cybernetics is used to delineate the epistemological field of system and cybernetic study. The above-mentioned phenomenological (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Descartes' psychology of vision and cognitive science: The optics (1637) in the light of Marr's (1982) vision.Geir kirkebøen - 1998 - Philosophical Psychology 11 (2):161 – 182.
    In this paper I consider the relation between Descartes' psychology of vision and the cognitive science approach to psychology (henceforth CS). In particular, I examine Descartes' the Optics (1637) in the light of David Marr's (1982) position in CS. My general claim is that CS can be seen as a rediscovery of Descartes' psychology of vision. In the first section, I point to a parallel between Descartes' epistemological revolution, which created the modem version of the problem of perception, and the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Computational modeling vs. computational explanation: Is everything a Turing machine, and does it matter to the philosophy of mind?Gualtiero Piccinini - 2007 - Australasian Journal of Philosophy 85 (1):93 – 115.
    According to pancomputationalism, everything is a computing system. In this paper, I distinguish between different varieties of pancomputationalism. I find that although some varieties are more plausible than others, only the strongest variety is relevant to the philosophy of mind, but only the most trivial varieties are true. As a side effect of this exercise, I offer a clarified distinction between computational modelling and computational explanation.<br><br>.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   46 citations  
  • What Neuroscientists Think, and Don’t Think, About Consciousness.Peter D. Kitchener & Colin G. Hales - 2022 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 16.
    The approach the majority of neuroscientists take to the question of how consciousness is generated, it is probably fair to say, is to ignore it. Although there are active research programs looking at correlates of consciousness, and explorations of informational properties of what might be relevant neural ensembles, the tacitly implied mechanism of consciousness in these approaches is that it somehow just happens. This reliance on a “magical emergence” of consciousness does not address the “objectively unreasonable” proposition that elements that (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unified cognitive theory: Having one's apple pie and eating it.Stephan Lewandowsky - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):449-450.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • On models and mechanisms.William R. Uttal - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):459-460.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cartesian vs. Newtonian research strategies for cognitive science.Morton E. Winston - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):463-464.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Active symbols, limited storage and the power of natural intelligence.Eric Chown & Stephen Kaplan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):442-443.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unified cognitive theory is not comprehensive.P. C. Dodwell - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):443-445.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Abstract codes are not just for chimpanzees.Thomas R. Zentall - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):157-158.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The last of Clever Hans?Derek Bickerton - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):141-142.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Resemblance and imaginal representation.Ned Block - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):142-143.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Images, labels, concepts, and propositions: Some reservations regarding Premack's “abstract code”.Arthur C. Danto - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):143-144.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Decision-making: from neuroscience to neuroeconomics—an overview.Daniel Serra - 2021 - Theory and Decision 91 (1):1-80.
    By the late 1990s, several converging trends in economics, psychology, and neuroscience had set the stage for the birth of a new scientific field known as “neuroeconomics”. Without the availability of an extensive variety of experimental designs for dealing with individual and social decision-making provided by experimental economics and psychology, many neuroeconomics studies could not have been developed. At the same time, without the significant progress made in neuroscience for grasping and understanding brain functioning, neuroeconomics would have never seen the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • How disunity matters to the history of cybernetics in the human sciences in the United States, 1940–80.Ronald Kline - 2020 - History of the Human Sciences 33 (1):12-35.
    Rather than assume a unitary cybernetics, I ask how its disunity mattered to the history of the human sciences in the United States from about 1940 to 1980. I compare the work of four prominent social scientists – Herbert Simon, George Miller, Karl Deutsch, and Talcott Parsons – who created cybernetic models in psychology, economics, political science, and sociology with the work of anthropologist Gregory Bateson, and relate their interpretations of cybernetics to those of such well-known cyberneticians as Norbert Wiener, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Plural Embodiment of Mind. Genealogy and Guidelines for a Radically Embodied Approach to Mind and Consciousness.Mauro Ceruti & Luisa Damiano - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Walter Pitts and “A Logical Calculus”.Mark Schlatter & Ken Aizawa - 2008 - Synthese 162 (2):235-250.
    Many years after the publication of “A Logical Calculus of the Ideas Immanent in Nervous Activity,” Warren McCulloch gave Walter Pitts credit for contributing his knowledge of modular mathematics to their joint project. In 1941 I presented my notions on the flow of information through ranks of neurons to Rashevsky’s seminar in the Committee on Mathematical Biology of the University of Chicago and met Walter Pitts, who then was about seventeen years old. He was working on a mathematical theory of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Précis of Unified theories of cognition.Allen Newell - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):425-437.
    The book presents the case that cognitive science should turn its attention to developing theories of human cognition that cover the full range of human perceptual, cognitive, and action phenomena. Cognitive science has now produced a massive number of high-quality regularities with many microtheories that reveal important mechanisms. The need for integration is pressing and will continue to increase. Equally important, cognitive science now has the theoretical concepts and tools to support serious attempts at unified theories. The argument is made (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Does language training affect the code used by chimpanzees?: Some cautions and reservations.H. L. Roitblat - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):155-156.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modules in models of memory.Donald E. Broadbent - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-94.
    This paper puts forward a general framework for thought about human information processing. It is intended to avoid some of the problems of pipeline or stage models of function. At the same time it avoids the snare of supposing a welter of indefinitely many separate processes. The approach is not particularly original, but rather represents the common elements or presuppositions in a number of modern theories. These presuppositions are not usually explicit, however, and making them so reduces the danger of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The embodiment of learning.Jim Horn & Denise Wilburn - 2005 - Educational Philosophy and Theory 37 (5):745–760.
    This paper offers an introduction to the philosophy and science of embodied learning, conceived as both the stabilizing and expansionary process that sustains order and novelty within learners’ worlds enacted through observing and describing. Embodied learning acknowledges stability and change as the purposeful conjoined characteristics that sustain learners. It is, in many respects, a composite theory that represents work from various disciplines. This ‘naturalized epistemology’ conceives a world of fact inevitably imbued with the values that our own structural histories guarantee (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Can indirect realism be demonstrated in the psychological laboratory?Stephen Wilcox & Stuart Katz - 1984 - Philosophy of the Social Sciences 14 (2):149-157.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Cognition and simulation.N. E. Wetherick - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):462-463.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Models as toothbrushes.Michael J. Watkins - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):86-86.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • How do representations get processed in real nerve cells?Gerald S. Wasserman - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):85-85.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The pros and cons of having a word for it.S. F. Walker - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):156-157.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On putting the cart before the horse: Taking perception seriously in unified theories of cognition.Kim J. Vicente & Alex Kirlik - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):461-462.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A cognitive process shell.Steven A. Vere - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):460-461.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Unified theories and theories that mimic each other's predictions.James T. Townsend - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):458-459.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Problem spaces, language and connectionism: Issues for cognition.Patrick Suppes - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):457-458.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Stage models of mental processing and the additive-factor method.Saul Sternberg - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):82-84.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A new abstract code or the new possibility of multiple codes?Annette Karmiloff Smith - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):149-150.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   59 citations  
  • Choosing a unifying theory for cognitive development.Thomas R. Shultz - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):456-457.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Practice, attention, and the processing system.Walter Schneider - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):80-81.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Does the evolutionary perspective offer more than constraints?Wolfgang Schleidt - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):456-456.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Information-flow diagrams as scientific models.Kenneth M. Sayre - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):79-80.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From AI to cybernetics.Keizo Sato - 1991 - AI and Society 5 (2):155-161.
    Well-known critics of AI such as Hubert Dreyfus and Michael Polanyi tend to confuse cybernetics with AI. Such a confusion is quite misleading and should not be overlooked. In the first place, cybernetics is not vulnerable to criticism of AI as cognitivistic and behaviouristic. In the second place, AI researchers are recommended to consider the cybernetics approach as a way of overcoming the limitations of cognitivism and behaviourism.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The use of interference paradigms as a criterion for separating memory stores.Henry L. Roediger - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):78-79.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Doubts about the importance of language training and the abstract code.William A. Roberts - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):154-155.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Simplistic heuristics and Maltese acrostics.Patrick Rabbitt - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):77-78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unified psychobiological theory.Duane Quiatt - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):454-455.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unified theories must explain the codependencies among perception, cognition and action.Robert W. Proctor & Addie Dutta - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):453-454.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The codes of man and beasts.David Premack - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):125-136.
    Exposing the chimpanzee to language training appears to enhance the animal's ability to perform some kinds of tasks but not others. The abilities that are enhanced involve abstract judgment, as in analogical reasoning, matching proportions of physically unlike exemplars, and completing incomplete representations of action. The abilities that do not improve concern the location of items in space and the inferences one might make in attempting to obtain them. Representing items in space and making inferences about them could be done (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   191 citations  
  • The abstract code as a translation device.David Premack - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):158-167.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark