Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The case for and difficulties in using “demand areas” to measure changes in well-being.Yew-Kwang Ng - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):30-31.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Animals, science, and morality.R. G. Frey - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):22-22.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • In defence of speciesism.J. A. Gray - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):22-23.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Dynamic models, fitness functions and food storing.Christine L. Hitchcock & David F. Sherry - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):99-99.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ethics and animals.Peter Singer - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):45-48.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Optimal confusion.Stephanie Stolarz-Fantino & Edmund Fantino - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):234-234.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The human being as a bumbling optimalist: A psychologist's viewpoint.Masanao Toda - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):235-235.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rational agents, real people and the quest for optimality.Eldar Shafir - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):232-232.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The infinite regress of optimization.Philippe Mongin - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):229-230.
    A comment on Paul Schoemaker's target article in Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14 (1991), p. 205-215, "The Quest for Optimality: A Positive Heuristic of Science?" (https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00066140). This comment argues that the optimizing model of decision leads to an infinite regress, once internal costs of decision (i.e., information and computation costs) are duly taken into account.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Some thinking is irrational.Jonathan Baron - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):486-487.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Does the environment have the same structure as Bayes' theorem?Gerd Gigerenzer - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):495-496.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Comparative cognition revisited.Stewart H. Hulse - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):379-379.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Representations and cognition.H. L. Roitblat - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):394-406.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Biological relevance.Howard Rachlin - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):144-144.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • State, function, and optimization.William A. Calder - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):131-133.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Self-control and the panda's thumb.Eliot Shimoff & A. Charles Catania - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):693-693.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On observing the unobservable.Ovide F. Pomerleau & Cynthia S. Pomerleau - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):692-692.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Evolutionary game theory: Suddenly it's 1960!John C. Malone - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):112.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dennett's rational animals: And how behavorism overlooked them.Ruth Garrett Millikan - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):372-373.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Average behaviorism is unedifying.William W. Rozeboom - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):712-714.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Teleological behaviorism and the intentional scheme.Hugh Lacey - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):134-135.
    Teleological behaviorism, unlike Skinnerian behaviorism, recognizes that are needed to account adequately for human behavior, but it rejects the essential role in behavioral explanations of the subjective perspective of the agent. I argue that teleological behaviorism fails because of this rejection.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Foraging for a science of behavior.Michael Davison - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):335-336.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The validation problem.Donald M. Wilkie - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):349-350.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pavlovian factors in choice behavior.Bruce L. Brown - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):333-333.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • An interdisciplinary approach to foraging behavior.Richard F. Green - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):338-338.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Outcome and mechanism in foraging.Roger L. Mellgren - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):344-345.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Altruism and selfishness.Howard Rachlin - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (2):239-250.
    Many situations in human life present choices between (a) narrowly preferred particular alternatives and (b) narrowly less preferred (or aversive) particular alternatives that nevertheless form part of highly preferred abstract behavioral patterns. Such alternatives characterize problems of self-control. For example, at any given moment, a person may accept alcoholic drinks yet also prefer being sober to being drunk over the next few days. Other situations present choices between (a) alternatives beneficial to an individual and (b) alternatives that are less beneficial (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Toward a better understanding of prosocial behavior: The role of evolution and directed attention.Stephen Kaplan & Raymond De Young - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (2):263-264.
    Rachlin's thought-provoking analysis could be strengthened by greater openness to evolutionary interpretation and the use of the directed attention concept as a component of self-control. His contribution to the understanding of prosocial behavior would also benefit from abandoning the traditional (and excessively restrictive) definition of altruism.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The 'panglossian paradigm' defended.Daniel C. Dennett - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):343-90.
    Ethologists and others studying animal behavior in a spirit are in need of a descriptive language and method that are neither anachronistically bound by behaviorist scruples nor prematurely committed to particular Just such an interim descriptive method can be found in intentional system theory. The use of intentional system theory is illustrated with the case of the apparently communicative behavior of vervet monkeys. A way of using the theory to generate data - including usable, testable data - is sketched. The (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   597 citations  
  • Science and subjective feelings.Dale Jamieson - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):25-26.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Consumer demand: Can we deal with differing priorities?P. Monaghan - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):29-30.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The “crooked bookie” cycle.F. J. Odling-Smee - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):103-103.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Rule of thumb.Jonathan Roughgarden - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):104-105.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Modeling change in biology and psychology.James T. Townsend - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (1):108-108.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Natural and unnatural justice in animal care.Stephen Walker - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):43-43.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On Singer: More argument, less prescriptivism.David DeGrazia - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (1):18-18.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • The quest for optimality: A positive heuristic of science?Paul J. H. Schoemaker - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (2):205-215.
    This paper examines the strengths and weaknesses of one of science's most pervasive and flexible metaprinciples;optimalityis used to explain utility maximization in economics, least effort principles in physics, entropy in chemistry, and survival of the fittest in biology. Fermat's principle of least time involves both teleological and causal considerations, two distinct modes of explanation resting on poorly understood psychological primitives. The rationality heuristic in economics provides an example from social science of the potential biases arising from the extreme flexibility of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Antimisrepresentationalism.A. Charles Catania - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):374-375.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The meaning of representation in animal memory.H. L. Roitblat - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):353-372.
    A representation is a remnant of previous experience that allows that experience to affect later behavior. This paper develops a metatheoretical view of representation and applies it to issues concerning representation in animals. To describe a representational system one must specify the following: thedomainor range of situations in the represented world to which the system applies; thecontentor set of features encoded and preserved by the system; thecodeor transformational rules relating features of the representation to the corresponding features of the represented (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   312 citations  
  • The functional analysis of behaviour: Making room for Prufrock.Felicity A. Huntingford & Neil B. Metcalfe - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):137-138.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Misinterpreting Mischel.Edmund J. S. Sonuga-Barke - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):693-694.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Evolution, behavior systems, and “self-control”: The fit between organism and test environment.William Timberlake - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):694-695.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the careful use of ecological models.Thomas Caraco - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):680-681.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Self-control in context.Leonard Green & Edwin B. Fisher - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (4):684-685.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Development and the origin of behavioral strategies.Timothy D. Johnston - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (1):108.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   53 citations  
  • Elementary errors about evolution.Richard C. Lewontin - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):367-368.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   60 citations  
  • Parlez-vous baboon, Bwana Sherlock?E. W. Menzel - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):371-372.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The International stance faces backward.Howard Rachlin - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):373-373.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does behaviorism explain self-control?Robert Eisenberger - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1):125-125.
    Rachlin's hyperbolic-discounting model captures basic features of the subtlety of human impulsiveness and self-control and has received convincing experimental support. His distinction between self-control patterns and impulsive acts expands his earlier work to a greater range of self-control behaviors. Possible mechanisms that may weaken or strengthen patterns of self-control are considered.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark