Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Time in the mind: Using space to think about time.Daniel Casasanto & Lera Boroditsky - 2008 - Cognition 106 (2):579-593.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   120 citations  
  • Six Views of Embodied Cognition.Margaret Wilson - 2002 - Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9 (4):625--636.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   368 citations  
  • Metaphors we live by.George Lakoff & Mark Johnson - 1980 - Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Edited by Mark Johnson.
    The now-classic Metaphors We Live By changed our understanding of metaphor and its role in language and the mind. Metaphor, the authors explain, is a fundamental mechanism of mind, one that allows us to use what we know about our physical and social experience to provide understanding of countless other subjects. Because such metaphors structure our most basic understandings of our experience, they are "metaphors we live by"--metaphors that can shape our perceptions and actions without our ever noticing them. In (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1160 citations  
  • Perceptual symbol systems.Lawrence W. Barsalou - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (4):577-660.
    Prior to the twentieth century, theories of knowledge were inherently perceptual. Since then, developments in logic, statis- tics, and programming languages have inspired amodal theories that rest on principles fundamentally different from those underlying perception. In addition, perceptual approaches have become widely viewed as untenable because they are assumed to implement record- ing systems, not conceptual systems. A perceptual theory of knowledge is developed here in the context of current cognitive science and neuroscience. During perceptual experience, association areas in the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   739 citations  
  • Metaphors We Live By.George Lakoff & Mark Johnson - 1980 - Ethics 93 (3):619-621.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1369 citations  
  • Metaphor: A Practical Introduction.Zoltan Kovecses - 2010 - New York: Oxford University Press.
    Combining up-to-date scholarship with clear and accessible language and helpful exercises, Metaphor: A Practical Introduction is an invaluable resource for all readers interested in metaphor. This second edition includes two new chapters--on 'metaphors in discourse' and 'metaphor and emotion' --along with new exercises, responses to criticism and recent developments in the field, and revised student exercises, tables, and figures.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   78 citations  
  • Philosophy in the Flesh: the Embodied Mind & its Challenge to Western Thought.George Lakoff (ed.) - 1999 - Basic Books.
    Reexamines the Western philosophical tradition, looking at the basic concepts of the mind, time, causation, morality, and the self.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   644 citations  
  • The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized.Wim Gevers, Bert Reynvoet & Wim Fias - 2003 - Cognition 87 (3):B87-B95.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   67 citations  
  • Time drawings: Spatial representation of temporal concepts.María Juliana Leone, Alejo Salles, Alejandro Pulver, Diego Andrés Golombek & Mariano Sigman - 2018 - Consciousness and Cognition 59:10-25.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Motor Action and Emotional Memory.Daniel Casasanto & Katinka Dijkstra - 2010 - Cognition 115 (1):179.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  • Metaphors We Live by.Max Black - 1980 - Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 40 (2):208-210.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   703 citations  
  • The mental representation of parity and number magnitude.Stanislas Dehaene, Serge Bossini & Pascal Giraux - 1993 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 122 (3):371–96.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   264 citations  
  • Vection modulates emotional valence of autobiographical episodic memories.Takeharu Seno, Takahiro Kawabe, Hiroyuki Ito & Shoji Sunaga - 2013 - Cognition 126 (1):115-120.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Visualizing Thought.Barbara Tversky - 2011 - Topics in Cognitive Science 3 (3):499-535.
    Depictive expressions of thought predate written language by thousands of years. They have evolved in communities through a kind of informal user testing that has refined them. Analyzing common visual communications reveals consistencies that illuminate how people think as well as guide design; the process can be brought into the laboratory and accelerated. Like language, visual communications abstract and schematize; unlike language, they use properties of the page (e.g., proximity and place: center, horizontal/up–down, vertical/left–right) and the marks on it (e.g., (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   39 citations  
  • How Linguistic and Cultural Forces Shape Conceptions of Time: English and Mandarin Time in 3D.Orly Fuhrman, Kelly McCormick, Eva Chen, Heidi Jiang, Dingfang Shu, Shuaimei Mao & Lera Boroditsky - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (7):1305-1328.
    In this paper we examine how English and Mandarin speakers think about time, and we test how the patterns of thinking in the two groups relate to patterns in linguistic and cultural experience. In Mandarin, vertical spatial metaphors are used more frequently to talk about time than they are in English; English relies primarily on horizontal terms. We present results from two tasks comparing English and Mandarin speakers’ temporal reasoning. The tasks measure how people spatialize time in three-dimensional space, including (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   33 citations  
  • Cross-Cultural Differences in Mental Representations of Time: Evidence From an Implicit Nonlinguistic Task.Orly Fuhrman & Lera Boroditsky - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (8):1430-1451.
    Across cultures people construct spatial representations of time. However, the particular spatial layouts created to represent time may differ across cultures. This paper examines whether people automatically access and use culturally specific spatial representations when reasoning about time. In Experiment 1, we asked Hebrew and English speakers to arrange pictures depicting temporal sequences of natural events, and to point to the hypothesized location of events relative to a reference point. In both tasks, English speakers (who read left to right) arranged (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   50 citations  
  • The Continuity of Metaphor: Evidence From Temporal Gestures.Esther Walker & Kensy Cooperrider - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (2):481-495.
    Reasoning about bedrock abstract concepts such as time, number, and valence relies on spatial metaphor and often on multiple spatial metaphors for a single concept. Previous research has documented, for instance, both future-in-front and future-to-right metaphors for time in English speakers. It is often assumed that these metaphors, which appear to have distinct experiential bases, remain distinct in online temporal reasoning. In two studies we demonstrate that, contra this assumption, people systematically combine these metaphors. Evidence for this combination was found (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Affect biases memory of location: Evidence for the spatial representation of affect.L. Elizabeth Crawford, Skye M. Margolies, John T. Drake & Meghan E. Murphy - 2006 - Cognition and Emotion 20 (8):1153-1169.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • The Role of Cultural Artifacts in the Interpretation of Metaphorical Expressions About Time.Sarah E. Duffy - 2014 - Metaphor and Symbol 29 (2):94-112.
    Across cultures, people employ space to construct representations of time. English exhibits two deictic space–time metaphors: the “moving ego” metaphor conceptualizes the ego as moving forward through time and the “moving time” metaphor conceptualizes time as moving forward towards the ego. Earlier research investigating the psychological reality of these metaphors has shown that engaging in certain types of spatial-motion thinking may influence how people reason about events in time. More recently, research has shown that people’s interactions with cultural artifacts may (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Latency of instrumental responses as a function of compatibility with the meaning of eliciting verbal signs.Andrew K. Solarz - 1960 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 59 (4):239.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   45 citations  
  • Does language shape thought?: Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time.Lera Boroditsky - 2001 - Cognitive Psychology 43:1-22.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   106 citations  
  • An effect of spatial–temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time.Antonino Vallesi, Malcolm A. Binns & Tim Shallice - 2008 - Cognition 107 (2):501-527.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Body-specific representations of spatial location.Tad T. Brunyé, Aaron Gardony, Caroline R. Mahoney & Holly A. Taylor - 2012 - Cognition 123 (2):229-239.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Handedness Shapes Children’s Abstract Concepts.Daniel Casasanto & Tania Henetz - 2012 - Cognitive Science 36 (2):359-372.
    Can children’s handedness influence how they represent abstract concepts like kindness and intelligence? Here we show that from an early age, right-handers associate rightward space more strongly with positive ideas and leftward space with negative ideas, but the opposite is true for left-handers. In one experiment, children indicated where on a diagram a preferred toy and a dispreferred toy should go. Right-handers tended to assign the preferred toy to a box on the right and the dispreferred toy to a box (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Horizontal spatial representations of time: evidence for the STEARC effect.Masami Ishihara, Peter Keller, Yves Rossetti & Wolfgang Prinz - 2008 - Cortex 44 (4):454–61.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations