Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. The mechanisms of human action: introduction and background.Ezequiel Morsella - 2008 - In Ezequiel Morsella, John A. Bargh & Peter M. Gollwitzer (eds.), Oxford handbook of human action. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 1--32.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Tip-of-the-tongue states reoccur because of implicit learning, but resolving them helps.Maria C. D’Angelo & Karin R. Humphreys - 2015 - Cognition 142 (C):166-190.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Now-or-Never bottleneck: A fundamental constraint on language.Morten H. Christiansen & Nick Chater - 2016 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39:e62.
    Memory is fleeting. New material rapidly obliterates previous material. How, then, can the brain deal successfully with the continual deluge of linguistic input? We argue that, to deal with this “Now-or-Never” bottleneck, the brain must compress and recode linguistic input as rapidly as possible. This observation has strong implications for the nature of language processing: (1) the language system must “eagerly” recode and compress linguistic input; (2) as the bottleneck recurs at each new representational level, the language system must build (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   76 citations  
  • Connectionist Natural Language Processing: The State of the Art.Morten H. Christiansen & Nick Chater - 1999 - Cognitive Science 23 (4):417-437.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Becoming syntactic.Franklin Chang, Gary S. Dell & Kathryn Bock - 2006 - Psychological Review 113 (2):234-272.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   120 citations  
  • Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement learning perspective.Matthew M. Botvinick, Yael Niv & Andrew C. Barto - 2009 - Cognition 113 (3):262-280.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   42 citations  
  • Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement learning perspective.Matthew M. Botvinick, Yael Niv & Andew G. Barto - 2009 - Cognition 113 (3):262-280.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • Doing Without Schema Hierarchies: A Recurrent Connectionist Approach to Normal and Impaired Routine Sequential Action.Matthew Botvinick & David C. Plaut - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (2):395-429.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   53 citations  
  • The prosodic domain of phonological encoding: Evidence from speech errors.Mary-Beth Beirne & Karen Croot - 2018 - Cognition 177 (C):1-7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The production of determiners: evidence from French.F. -Xavier Alario & Alfonso Caramazza - 2002 - Cognition 82 (3):179-223.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Similarity of referents influences the learning of phonological word forms: Evidence from concurrent word learning.Libo Zhao, Stephanie Packard, Bob McMurray & Prahlad Gupta - 2019 - Cognition 190 (C):42-60.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Serial order in phonological encoding: an exploration of the 'word onset effect' using laboratory-induced errors.C. Wilshire - 1998 - Cognition 68 (2):143-166.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Syntactic structure assembly in human parsing: a computational model based on competitive inhibition and a lexicalist grammar.Theo Vosse & Gerard Kempen - 2000 - Cognition 75 (2):105-143.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   31 citations  
  • The developing cognitive substrate of sequential action control in 9- to 12-month-olds: Evidence for concurrent activation models. [REVIEW]S. A. Verschoor, M. Paulus, M. Spapé, S. Biro & B. Hommel - 2015 - Cognition 138:64-78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Understanding the Phonetic Characteristics of Speech Under Uncertainty—Implications of the Representation of Linguistic Knowledge in Learning and Processing.Fabian Tomaschek & Michael Ramscar - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    The uncertainty associated with paradigmatic families has been shown to correlate with their phonetic characteristics in speech, suggesting that representations of complex sublexical relations between words are part of speaker knowledge. To better understand this, recent studies have used two-layer neural network models to examine the way paradigmatic uncertainty emerges in learning. However, to date this work has largely ignored the way choices about the representation of inflectional and grammatical functions in models strongly influence what they subsequently learn. To explore (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • High level processing scope in spoken sentence production.Mark Smith & Linda Wheeldon - 1999 - Cognition 73 (3):205-246.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Memory and cognitive control in an integrated theory of language processing.L. Robert Slevc & Jared M. Novick - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (4):373-374.
    Pickering & Garrod's (P&G's) integrated model of production and comprehension includes no explicit role for nonlinguistic cognitive processes. Yet, how domain-general cognitive functions contribute to language processing has become clearer with well-specified theories and supporting data. We therefore believe that their account can benefit by incorporating functions like working memory and cognitive control into a unified model of language processing.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Speed, Accuracy, and Serial Order in Sequence Production.Peter Q. Pfordresher, Caroline Palmer & Melissa K. Jungers - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (1):63-98.
    The production of complex sequences like music or speech requires the rapid and temporally precise production of events (e.g., notes and chords), often at fast rates. Memory retrieval in these circumstances may rely on the simultaneous activation of both the current event and the surrounding context (Lashley, 1951). We describe an extension to a model of incremental retrieval in sequence production (Palmer & Pfordresher, 2003) that incorporates this logic to predict overall error rates and speed—accuracy trade-offs, as well as types (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Forward models and their implications for production, comprehension, and dialogue.Martin J. Pickering & Simon Garrod - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (4):377-392.
    Our target article proposed that language production and comprehension are interwoven, with speakers making predictions of their own utterances and comprehenders making predictions of other people's utterances at different linguistic levels. Here, we respond to comments about such issues as cognitive architecture and its neural basis, learning and development, monitoring, the nature of forward models, communicative intentions, and dialogue.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • An integrated theory of language production and comprehension.Martin J. Pickering & Simon Garrod - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (4):329-347.
    Currently, production and comprehension are regarded as quite distinct in accounts of language processing. In rejecting this dichotomy, we instead assert that producing and understanding are interwoven, and that this interweaving is what enables people to predict themselves and each other. We start by noting that production and comprehension are forms of action and action perception. We then consider the evidence for interweaving in action, action perception, and joint action, and explain such evidence in terms of prediction. Specifically, we assume (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   157 citations  
  • Speed, Accuracy, and Serial Order in Sequence Production.Peter Q. Pfordresher, Caroline Palmer & Melissa K. Jungers - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (1):63-98.
    The production of complex sequences like music or speech requires the rapid and temporally precise production of events (e.g., notes and chords), often at fast rates. Memory retrieval in these circumstances may rely on the simultaneous activation of both the current event and the surrounding context (Lashley, 1951). We describe an extension to a model of incremental retrieval in sequence production (Palmer & Pfordresher, 2003) that incorporates this logic to predict overall error rates and speed—accuracy trade-offs, as well as types (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Incremental planning in sequence production.Caroline Palmer & Peter Q. Pfordresher - 2003 - Psychological Review 110 (4):683-712.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Within-word serial order control: Adjacent mora exchange and serial position effects in repeated single-word production.Masataka Nakayama & Satoru Saito - 2014 - Cognition 131 (3):415-430.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Similarities between Cognitive Models of Language Production and Everyday Functioning: Implications for Development of Interventions for Functional Difficulties.Rachel Mis & Tania Giovannetti - 2022 - Topics in Cognitive Science 14 (2):295-310.
    Topics in Cognitive Science, Volume 14, Issue 2, Page 295-310, April 2022.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Effects of Speech Rate and Practice on the Allocation of Visual Attention in Multiple Object Naming.Antje S. Meyer, Linda Wheeldon, Femke van der Meulen & Agnieszka Konopka - 2012 - Frontiers in Psychology 3.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement-learning perspective.Andrew C. Barto Matthew M. Botvinick, Yael Niv - 2009 - Cognition 113 (3):262.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • Context and meter enhance long-range planning in music performance.Brian Mathias, Peter Q. Pfordresher & Caroline Palmer - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996).Maryellen C. MacDonald & Morten H. Christiansen - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (1):35-54.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   55 citations  
  • Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations.Gordon D. Logan & Robert D. Gordon - 2001 - Psychological Review 108 (2):393-434.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   68 citations  
  • An activation‐based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval.Richard L. Lewis & Shravan Vasishth - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (3):375-419.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   133 citations  
  • A theory of lexical access in speech production.Willem J. M. Levelt, Ardi Roelofs & Antje S. Meyer - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (1):1-38.
    Preparing words in speech production is normally a fast and accurate process. We generate them two or three per second in fluent conversation; and overtly naming a clear picture of an object can easily be initiated within 600 msec after picture onset. The underlying process, however, is exceedingly complex. The theory reviewed in this target article analyzes this process as staged and feedforward. After a first stage of conceptual preparation, word generation proceeds through lexical selection, morphological and phonological encoding, phonetic (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   266 citations  
  • Emergence of an Action Repository as Part of a Biologically Inspired Model of Speech Processing: The Role of Somatosensory Information in Learning Phonetic-Phonological Sound Features.Bernd J. Kröger, Tanya Bafna & Mengxue Cao - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Repeated speech errors: Evidence for learning.Karin R. Humphreys, Heather Menzies & Johanna K. Lake - 2010 - Cognition 117 (2):151-165.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Consequences of the Serial Nature of Linguistic Input for Sentenial Complexity.Daniel Grodner & Edward Gibson - 2005 - Cognitive Science 29 (2):261-290.
    All other things being equal the parser favors attaching an ambiguous modifier to the most recent possible site. A plausible explanation is that locality preferences such as this arise in the service of minimizing memory costs—more distant sentential material is more difficult to reactivate than more recent material. Note that processing any sentence requires linking each new lexical item with material in the current parse. This often involves the construction of long‐distance dependencies. Under a resource‐limited view of language processing, lengthy (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   48 citations  
  • Learning to divide the labor: an account of deficits in light and heavy verb production.Jean K. Gordon & Gary S. Dell - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (1):1-40.
    Theories of sentence production that involve a convergence of activation from conceptual‐semantic and syntactic‐sequential units inspired a connectionist model that was trained to produce simple sentences. The model used a learning algorithm that resulted in a sharing of responsibility (or “division of labor”) between syntactic and semantic inputs for lexical activation according to their predictive power. Semantically rich, or “heavy”, verbs in the model came to rely on semantic cues more than on syntactic cues, whereas semantically impoverished, or “light”, verbs (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Does bilingualism twist your tongue?Tamar H. Gollan & Matthew Goldrick - 2012 - Cognition 125 (3):491-497.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Hierarchy and scope of planning in subject–verb agreement production.Maureen Gillespie & Neal J. Pearlmutter - 2011 - Cognition 118 (3):377-397.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • The dark side of incremental learning: A model of cumulative semantic interference during lexical access in speech production.Myrna F. Schwartz Gary M. Oppenheim, Gary S. Dell - 2010 - Cognition 114 (2):227.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  • Representation of letter position in spelling: Evidence from acquired dysgraphia.Simon Fischer-Baum, Michael McCloskey & Brenda Rapp - 2010 - Cognition 115 (3):466-490.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Skill acquisition in music performance: relations between planning and temporal control.Carolyn Drake & Caroline Palmer - 2000 - Cognition 74 (1):1-32.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Psycholinguistics, computational.Richard L. Lewis - 2003 - In L. Nadel (ed.), Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Nature Publishing Group.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Toward a language-general account of word production: The proximate units principle.Padraig G. O'Seaghdha & Jenn-Yeu Chen - 2009 - In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. pp. 68--73.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Brain activations during conscious self-monitoring of speech production with delayed auditory feedback: An fMRI study.Yasuki Hashimoto & Kuniyoshi L. Sakai - 2003 - Human Brain Mapping 20 (1):22-28.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations