Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Strategies for the control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom.Gerald L. Gottlieb, Daniel M. Corcos & Gyan C. Agarwal - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):189-210.
    A theory is presented to explain how accurate, single-joint movements are controlled. The theory applies to movements across different distances, with different inertial loads, toward targets of different widths over a wide range of experimentally manipulated velocities. The theory is based on three propositions. (1) Movements are planned according to “strategies” of which there are at least two: a speed-insensitive (SI) and a speed-sensitive (SS) one. (2) These strategies can be equated with sets of rules for performing diverse movement tasks. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   183 citations  
  • The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex.J. F. Stein - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):691-700.
    The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is the most likely site where egocentric spatial relationships are represented in the brain. PPC cells receive visual, auditory, somaesthetic, and vestibular sensory inputs; oculomotor, head, limb, and body motor signals; and strong motivational projections from the limbic system. Their discharge increases not only when an animal moves towards a sensory target, but also when it directs its attention to it. PPC lesions have the opposite effect: sensory inattention and neglect. The PPC does not seem (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   87 citations  
  • Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements?S. C. Gandevia & David Burke - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):614-632.
    This target article draws together two groups of experimental studies on the control of human movement through peripheral feedback and centrally generated signals of motor commands. First, during natural movement, feedback from muscle, joint, and cutaneous afferents changes; in human subjects these changes have reflex and kinesthetic consequences. Recent psychophysical and microneurographic evidence suggests that joint and even cutaneous afferents may have a proprioceptive role. Second, the role of centrally generated motor commands in the control of normal movements and movements (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   82 citations  
  • The origin and use of positional frames of reference in motor control.Anatol G. Feldman & Mindy F. Levin - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):723-744.
    A hypothesis about sensorimotor integration (the λ model) is described and applied to movement control and kinesthesia. The central idea is that the nervous system organizes positional frames of reference for the sensorimotor apparatus and produces active movements by shifting the frames in terms of spatial coordinates. Kinematic and electromyographic patterns are not programmed, but emerge from the dynamic interaction among the system s components, including external forces within the designated frame of reference. Motoneuronal threshold properties and proprioceptive inputs to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   48 citations  
  • Does the nervous system use equilibrium-point control to guide single and multiple joint movements?E. Bizzi, N. Hogan, F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi & S. Giszter - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):603-613.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   48 citations  
  • SOAR as a unified theory of cognition: Issues and explanations.Allen Newell - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):464-492.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   44 citations  
  • From Universal Laws of Cognition to Specific Cognitive Models.Nick Chater & Gordon D. A. Brown - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (1):36-67.
    The remarkable successes of the physical sciences have been built on highly general quantitative laws, which serve as the basis for understanding an enormous variety of specific physical systems. How far is it possible to construct universal principles in the cognitive sciences, in terms of which specific aspects of perception, memory, or decision making might be modelled? Following Shepard (e.g., ), it is argued that some universal principles may be attainable in cognitive science. Here, 2 examples are proposed: the simplicity (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  • How does the nervous system control the equilibrium trajectory?S. V. Adamovich - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):704-705.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Is stiffness the mainspring of posture and movement?Z. Hasan - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):756-758.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Strategies for the control of studies of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom.Gerale E. Loeb - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):227-227.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Equilibrium-point hypothesis, minimum effort control strategy and the triphasic muscle activation pattern.Ning Lan & Patrick E. Crago - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):769-771.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • Different regions of space or different spaces altogether: What are the dorsal/ventral systems processing?Gary W. Strong - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):556-557.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in humans: Its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications.Fred H. Previc - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):519-542.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Scale-invariance as a unifying psychological principle.Nick Chater & Gordon D. A. Brown - 1999 - Cognition 69 (3):B17-B24.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   16 citations  
  • Précis of Unified theories of cognition.Allen Newell - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):425-437.
    The book presents the case that cognitive science should turn its attention to developing theories of human cognition that cover the full range of human perceptual, cognitive, and action phenomena. Cognitive science has now produced a massive number of high-quality regularities with many microtheories that reveal important mechanisms. The need for integration is pressing and will continue to increase. Equally important, cognitive science now has the theoretical concepts and tools to support serious attempts at unified theories. The argument is made (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Differential effects of a visual illusion on online visual guidance in a stable environment and online adjustments to perturbations.Simone R. Caljouw, John van der Kamp, Moniek Lijster & Geert J. P. Savelsbergh - 2011 - Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4):1135-1143.
    In the reported, experiment participants hit a ball to aim at the vertex of a Müller–Lyer configuration. This configuration either remained stable, changed its shaft length or the orientation of the tails during movement execution. A significant illusion bias was observed in all perturbation conditions, but not in the stationary condition. The illusion bias emerged for perturbations shortly after movement onset and for perturbations during execution, the latter of which allowed only a minimum of time for making adjustments . These (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Search Strategies in the Perceptual-Motor Workspace and the Acquisition of Coordination, Control, and Skill.Matheus M. Pacheco, Charley W. Lafe & Karl M. Newell - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Variability of Practice, Information Processing, and Decision Making—How Much Do We Know?Stanisław H. Czyż - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Decision-making is a complex action requiring efficient information processing. Specifically, in movement in which performance efficiency depends on reaction time, e.g., open-loop controlled movements, these processes may play a crucial role. Information processing includes three distinct stages, stimulus identification, response selection, and response programming. Mainly, response selection may play a substantial contribution to the reaction time and appropriate decision making. The duration of this stage depends on the number of possible choices an individual has to “screen” to make a proper (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The prerequisites for one-jint motor control theories.S. V. Adamovich & A. G. Feldman - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):210-211.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Direct pattern-imposing control or dynamic regulation?Marl L. Latash - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):226-227.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Braking may be more critical than acceleration.William A. MacKay - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (2):227-228.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Approximate Optimal Control as a Model for Motor Learning.Neil E. Berthier, Michael T. Rosenstein & Andrew G. Barto - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (2):329-346.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Implications of neural networks for how we think about brain function.David A. Robinson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):644-655.
    Engineers use neural networks to control systems too complex for conventional engineering solutions. To examine the behavior of individual hidden units would defeat the purpose of this approach because it would be largely uninterpretable. Yet neurophysiologists spend their careers doing just that! Hidden units contain bits and scraps of signals that yield only arcane hints about network function and no information about how its individual units process signals. Most literature on single-unit recordings attests to this grim fact. On the other (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Quantifying the Ebbinghaus figure effect: target size, context size, and target-context distance determine the presence and direction of the illusion.Hester Knol, Raoul Huys, Jean-Christophe Sarrazin & Viktor K. Jirsa - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Visual processing in three-dimensional space: Perceptions and misperceptions.Fred H. Previc - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):559-575.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Grasping time does not influence the early adherence of aperture shaping to Weber's law.Matthew Heath, Scott A. Holmes, Ali Mulla & Gordon Binsted - 2012 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Pantomime-Grasping: Advance Knowledge of Haptic Feedback Availability Supports an Absolute Visuo-Haptic Calibration.Shirin Davarpanah Jazi & Matthew Heath - 2016 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • SOAR as a world view, not a theory.Earl Hunt & R. Duncan Luce - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):447-448.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • A cognitive process shell.Steven A. Vere - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):460-461.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Are perceptuo-motor decisions really more optimal than cognitive decisions?Andreas Jarvstad, Ulrike Hahn, Paul A. Warren & Simon K. Rushton - 2014 - Cognition 130 (3):397-416.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Effects of angular gain transformations between movement and visual feedback on coordination performance in unimanual circling.Martina Rieger, Sandra Dietrich & Wolfgang Prinz - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • The Development of Generalized Motor Program in Constant and Variable Practice Conditions.Stanisław H. Czyż, Martin Zvonař & Elric Pretorius - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • (1 other version)Speed, Accuracy, and Serial Order in Sequence Production.Peter Q. Pfordresher, Caroline Palmer & Melissa K. Jungers - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (1):63-98.
    The production of complex sequences like music or speech requires the rapid and temporally precise production of events (e.g., notes and chords), often at fast rates. Memory retrieval in these circumstances may rely on the simultaneous activation of both the current event and the surrounding context (Lashley, 1951). We describe an extension to a model of incremental retrieval in sequence production (Palmer & Pfordresher, 2003) that incorporates this logic to predict overall error rates and speed—accuracy trade-offs, as well as types (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • (1 other version)Economy of Effort or Maximum Rate of Information? Exploring Basic Principles of Articulatory Dynamics.Yi Xu & Santitham Prom-on - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Systematic error in the organization of physical action.Charles B. Walter, Stephan P. Swinnen, Natalia Dounskaia & H. Van Langendonk - 2001 - Cognitive Science 25 (3):393-422.
    Current views of the control of complex, purposeful movements acknowledge that organizational processes must reconcile multiple concerns. The central priority is of course accomplishing the actor's goal. But in specifying the manner in which this occurs, the action plan must accommodate such factors as the interaction of mechanical forces associated with the motion of a multilinked system (classical mechanics) and, in many cases, intrinsic bias toward preferred movement patterns, characterized by so‐called “coordination dynamics.” The most familiar example of the latter (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Sensory Integration during Vibration of Postural Muscle Tendons When Pointing to a Memorized Target.Normand Teasdale, Mariusz P. Furmanek, Mathieu Germain Robitaille, Fabio Carlos Lucas de Oliveira & Martin Simoneau - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Neural Control Adaptation to Motor Noise Manipulation.Christopher J. Hasson, Olga Gelina & Garrett Woo - 2016 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The cerebellum and memory.Richard F. Thompson - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):801-802.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Systematic error in the organization of physical action.C. B. Walter, S. P. Swinnen, N. Dounskaia & H. Langendonk - 2001 - Cognitive Science 25 (3):393-422.
    Current views of the control of complex, purposeful movements acknowledge that organizational processes must reconcile multiple concerns. The central priority is of course accomplishing the actor's goal. But in specifying the manner in which this occurs, the action plan must accommodate such factors as the interaction of mechanical forces associated with the motion of a multilinked system (classical mechanics) and, in many cases, intrinsic bias toward preferred movement patterns, characterized by so-called “coordination dynamics.” The most familiar example of the latter (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Interaction between physiological and subjective states predicts the effect of a judging panel on the postures of cellists in performance.Satoshi Endo - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Intra-Auditory Integration Improves Motor Performance and Synergy in an Accurate Multi-Finger Pressing Task.Kyung Koh, Hyun Joon Kwon, Yang Sun Park, Tim Kiemel, Ross H. Miller, Yoon Hyuk Kim, Joon-Ho Shin & Jae Kun Shim - 2016 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Does visual-field specialization really have implications for coordinated visual-motor behavior?Richard A. Abrams - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):542-543.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Upper Limb Asymmetry in the Sense of Effort Is Dependent on Force Level.Diane E. Adamo, Mark Mitchell & Bernard J. Martin - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Tendon elasticity and positional control.R. McN Alexander - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-745.
    The spring-like behaviour of a joint following a sudden change of torque is partly a result of the elastic properties of tendons. A large fall in a muscle with a long tendon may be accompanied by tendon recoil causing joint movements as large as 20°.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • APAs Constraints to Voluntary Movements: The Case for Limb Movements Coupling.Fausto G. Baldissera & Luigi Tesio - 2017 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 11.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unified cognitive theory: You can't get there from here.Derek Bickerton - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):437-438.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is the multi-joint pointing movement model applicable to equilibrium control during upper trunk movements?Alexey Alexandrov, Alexander Frolov & Jean Massion - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-746.
    Two aspects of the target article, (1) the extension of the equilibrium point theory to multi-joint movements, and (2) the consequence that the EMG pattern is not directly controlled by the central nervous system (CNS), are discussed in light of the experiments on upper trunk bending in humans. The principle component kinematic analysis and the analysis of the EMG data, obtained under microgravity and additional loading conditions, support the application of Feldman and Levin's for multi-joint pointing movement to equilibrium control (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A few reasons why psychologlsts can adhere to Feldman and Levin's model.Mireille Bonnard & Jean Pailhous - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):746-747.
    We emphasize the relevance to cognitive psychology of Feldman and Levin's theoretical position. Traditional views of motor control have failed to clearly separate “production control” at the level of motor command, based on task-independent CV (control variables), from intentional “product control” based on task-dependent parameters. Because F&L's approach concentrates on the first process (trajectory formation), it can distinguish the product control stage.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Seeing double: Dichotomizing the visual system.R. Martyn Bracewell - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):543-544.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The benefits and constraints of visual processing dichotomies.Julie R. Brannan - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (3):544-545.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark