Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Nonsynaptic interactions in Aplysia and their relation to vertebrate systems.W. Dale Branton & Earl Mayeri - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):419-419.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Spatial analysis of brain function:Not the first.Robert M. Boynton - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):175-175.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Perhaps it's time to completely rethink cerebellar function.James M. Bower - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):438-439.
    The primary assumption made in this series of target articles is that the cerebellum is directly involved in motor control. However, in my opinion, there is ample and growing experimental evidence to question this classical view, whether or not learning is involved. I propose, instead, that the cerebellum is involved in the control of data acquisition for many different sensory systems, [CRÉPEL et al., HOUK et al., SMITH, THACH].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The trace amines: neurohumors (cytosolic, pre- and/or post-synaptic, secondary, indirect)?Alan A. Boulton - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):418-418.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • B. F. Skinner: A dissident view.Kenneth E. Boulding - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):483-484.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A few reasons why psychologlsts can adhere to Feldman and Levin's model.Mireille Bonnard & Jean Pailhous - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):746-747.
    We emphasize the relevance to cognitive psychology of Feldman and Levin's theoretical position. Traditional views of motor control have failed to clearly separate “production control” at the level of motor command, based on task-independent CV (control variables), from intentional “product control” based on task-dependent parameters. Because F&L's approach concentrates on the first process (trajectory formation), it can distinguish the product control stage.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the status of causal modes.Robert C. Bolles - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):482-483.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • On a clear day you can see behavior.Robert C. Bolles - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):619-620.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Central command and reflex regulation: Cardiovascular patterns during behavior.Alberto Del Bo & Alberto Zanchetti - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):297-298.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Consciousness and accessibility.Ned Block - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (4):596-598.
    This is my first publication of the distinction between phenomenal consciousness and access consciousness, though not using quite those terms. It ends with this: "The upshot is this: If Searle is using the access sense of "consciousness," his argument doesn't get to first base. If, as is more likely, he intends the what-it-is-like sense, his argument depends on assumptions about issues that the cognitivist is bound to regard as deeply unsettled empirical questions." Searle replies: "He refers to what he calls (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   125 citations  
  • Does the nervous system use equilibrium-point control to guide single and multiple joint movements?E. Bizzi, N. Hogan, F. A. Mussa-Ivaldi & S. Giszter - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):603-613.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • A nonPavlovian view of Pavlovian conditioning.Dalbir Bindra - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):632-634.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Behavioral stress and myocardial ischemia: An example of conditional response modification.George E. Billman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):295-296.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • In search of the theoretical basis of motor control.M. B. Berkinblit, A. G. Feldman & O. I. Fukson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):626-638.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Comment: Laterality and Evaluative Bivalence: A Neuroevolutionary Perspective.Gary G. Berntson, Greg J. Norman & John T. Cacioppo - 2011 - Emotion Review 3 (3):344-346.
    Rutherford and Lindell (2011) review an extensive literature on lateralization of emotion. As they note, an important issue surrounding this question is the nature of emotion, which bears on what, precisely, is lateralized. The present comments are intended to broaden the context of the review, by considering lateralization from the standpoint of a bivariate model of evaluative processes and a neuroevolutionary perspective.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Cardiovascular behaviour: Where does it take us?D. H. Bergel - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):295-295.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms.M. B. Berkinblit, A. G. Feldman & O. I. Fukson - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):585-599.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   167 citations  
  • Autonomic determinism: The modes of autonomic control, the doctrine of autonomic space, and the laws of autonomic constraint.Gary G. Berntson, John T. Cacioppo & Karen S. Quigley - 1991 - Psychological Review 98 (4):459-487.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • What has to be learned in motor learning?Harold Bekkering, Detlef Heck & Fahad Sultan - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):436-437.
    The present commentary considers the question of what must be learned in different types of motor skills, thereby limiting the question of what should be adjusted in the APG model in order to explain successful learning. It is concluded that an open loop model like the APG might well be able to describe the learning pattern of motor skills in a stable, predictable environment. Recent research on saccadic plasticity, however, illustrates that motor skills performed in an unpredictable environment depend heavily (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Force as the controlling muscle variable in limb movement.P. N. S. Bawa & J. Dickinson - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):543-543.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Flow diagrams and hydraulic models.Patrick Bateson - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):382.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Skinner on selection – A case study of intellectual isolation.George W. Barlow - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):481-482.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Intercellular communication in the CNS.Jeffery L. Barker - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):417-418.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Chaos, symbols, and connectionism.John A. Barnden - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):174-175.
    The paper is a commentary on the target article by Christine A. Skarda & Walter J. Freeman, “How brains make chaos in order to make sense of the world”, in the same issue of the journal, pp.161–195. -/- I confine my comments largely to some philosophical claims that Skarda & Freeman make and to the relationship of their model to connectionism. Some of the comments hinge on what symbols are and how they might sit in neural systems.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Chaotic dynamics in brain activity.A. Babloyantz - 1987 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10 (2):173-174.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pavlov's concept of reinforcement.E. A. Asratyan - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (4):631-632.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The problem of nonsynaptic transmission in the neostriatum.M. Arluison - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):416-417.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Terminology, modes of communication, and a command neurohormone.S. Arch - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (3):416-416.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Spanning the levels in cerebellar function.Michael A. Arbib - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (3):434-435.
    We ask what cerebellum and basal ganglia arguing that cerebellum tunes motor schemas and their coordination. We argue for a synthesis of models addressing the real-time role and error signaling roles of climbing fibers. bridges between regional and neuro-physiological studies, while relates the neurochemis-try of learning to neural and behavioral levels. [CRÉPEL et al.; HOUK et al.; KANO; LINDEN; SIMPSON et al.; SMITH; THACH; VINCENT].
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuroethology: A call for less exclusivity and more theory.Michael A. Arbib - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (3):381.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A new synthesis?Michael A. Arbib - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (4):619-619.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Classification of afferents by input not by output?P. L. R. Andrews & I. N. C. Lawes - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):300-301.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Conscious thoughts from reflex-like processes: A new experimental paradigm for consciousness research.Allison K. Allen, Kevin Wilkins, Adam Gazzaley & Ezequiel Morsella - 2013 - Consciousness and Cognition 22 (4):1318-1331.
    The contents of our conscious mind can seem unpredictable, whimsical, and free from external control. When instructed to attend to a stimulus in a work setting, for example, one might find oneself thinking about household chores. Conscious content thus appears different in nature from reflex action. Under the appropriate conditions, reflexes occur predictably, reliably, and via external control. Despite these intuitions, theorists have proposed that, under certain conditions, conscious content resembles reflexes and arises reliably via external control. We introduce the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Tendon elasticity and positional control.R. McN Alexander - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-745.
    The spring-like behaviour of a joint following a sudden change of torque is partly a result of the elastic properties of tendons. A large fall in a muscle with a long tendon may be accompanied by tendon recoil causing joint movements as large as 20°.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Servos and regulators in the control of leg muscles.R. McN Alexander - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):542-542.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is the multi-joint pointing movement model applicable to equilibrium control during upper trunk movements?Alexey Alexandrov, Alexander Frolov & Jean Massion - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):745-746.
    Two aspects of the target article, (1) the extension of the equilibrium point theory to multi-joint movements, and (2) the consequence that the EMG pattern is not directly controlled by the central nervous system (CNS), are discussed in light of the experiments on upper trunk bending in humans. The principle component kinematic analysis and the analysis of the EMG data, obtained under microgravity and additional loading conditions, support the application of Feldman and Levin's for multi-joint pointing movement to equilibrium control (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do legs have surplus degrees of freedom?R. McN Alexander - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):600-600.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Complexity in control of movements.Gyan C. Agarwal & Gerald L. Gottlieb - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):599-600.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How does the nervous system control the equilibrium trajectory?S. V. Adamovich - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (4):704-705.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   40 citations  
  • A speech-motor-system perspective on nervous-system-control variables.James H. Abbs - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):541-542.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Action.Elisabeth Pacherie - 2012 - In Keith Frankish & William Ramsey (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 92--111.
    In recent years, the integration of philosophical with scientific theorizing has started to yield new insights. This chapter surveys some recent philosophical and empirical work on the nature and structure of action, on conscious agency, and on our knowledge of actions.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Function and Teleology.Justin Garson - 2008 - In Sahorta Sarkar & Anya Plutynski (eds.), Companion to the Philosophy of Biology. Blackwell. pp. 525-549.
    This is a short overview of the biological functions debate in philosophy. While it was fairly comprehensive when it was written, my short book ​A Critical Overview of Biological Functions has largely supplanted it as a definitive and up-to-date overview of the debate, both because the book takes into account new developments since then, and because the length of the book allowed me to go into substantially more detail about existing views.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • What muscle variable(s) does the nervous system control in limb movements?R. B. Stein - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (4):535-541.
    To controlforceaccurately under a wide range of behavioral conditions, the central nervous system would either require a detailed, continuously updated representation of the state of each muscle (and the load against which each is acting) or else force feedback with sufficient gain to cope with variations in the properties of the muscles and loads. The evidence for force feedback with adequate gain or for an appropriate central representation is not sufficient to conclude that force is the major controlled variable in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   168 citations  
  • An essay on the circulation as behavior.Bernard T. Engel - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (2):285-295.
    Most conceptual models of the organization of the cardiovascular system begin with the premise that the nervous system regulates the metabolic and nonmetabolic reflex adjustments of the circulation. These models assume that all the neurally mediated responses of the circulation are reactive, i.e., reflexes elicited by adequate stimuli. This target article suggests that the responses of the circulation are conditional in three senses. First, as Sherrington argued, reflexes are conditional in that they never operate in a vacuum but in a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Giving up the ghost.William Vaughan - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):501-501.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Selection by consequences: A universal causal mode?William Timberlake - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):499-501.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Perspectives by consequences.Duane M. Rumbaugh - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):496-497.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Group and individual effects in selection.Marvin Harris - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):490-491.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The emancipation of thought and culture from their original material substrates.Michael T. Ghiselin - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):489-489.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Skinner – The Darwin of ontogeny?John W. Donahoe - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):487-488.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation