Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. On putting the cart before the horse: Taking perception seriously in unified theories of cognition.Kim J. Vicente & Alex Kirlik - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):461-462.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Architecture of the mind and libertarian paternalism: is the reversibility of system 1 nudges likely to happen?Riccardo Viale - 2019 - Mind and Society 18 (2):143-166.
    The libertarian attribute of Thaler and Sunstein’s nudge theory (Nudge: improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. Yale University Press, New Haven, 2008) is one of the most important features for its candidature as a new model for public policy-making. It relies on the reversibility of choices made under the influence of nudging. Since the mind is articulated into two systems, the choice taken by System 1 is always reversible because it can be overridden by the deliberative and corrective role (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Top-down fast-same, and acoustic perception.Rolf Verleger - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):257-258.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Näätänen's auditory model from a visual perspective.Marinus N. Verbaten - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):256-257.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning to apply theory of mind.Rineke Verbrugge & Lisette Mol - 2008 - Journal of Logic, Language and Information 17 (4):489-511.
    In everyday life it is often important to have a mental model of the knowledge, beliefs, desires, and intentions of other people. Sometimes it is even useful to to have a correct model of their model of our own mental states: a second-order Theory of Mind. In order to investigate to what extent adults use and acquire complex skills and strategies in the domains of Theory of Mind and the related skill of natural language use, we conducted an experiment. It (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Intelligence: Some neglected topics.Philip E. Vernon - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):302.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Evidence for capacity sharing when stopping.Frederick Verbruggen & Gordon D. Logan - 2015 - Cognition 142 (C):81-95.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A cognitive process shell.Steven A. Vere - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):460-461.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • An atropine-sensitive and a less atropine-sensitive system.Robert P. Vertes - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):493-494.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The limits of neuropsychological models of consciousness.Max Velmans - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):702-703.
    This commentary elaborates on Gray's conclusion that his neurophysiological model of consciousness might explain how consciousness arises from the brain, but does not address how consciousness evolved, affects behaviour or confers survival value. The commentary argues that such limitations apply to all neurophysiological or other third-person perspective models. To approach such questions the first-person nature of consciousness needs to be taken seriously in combination with third-person models of the brain.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Is human information processing conscious?Max Velmans - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):651-69.
    Investigations of the function of consciousness in human information processing have focused mainly on two questions: (1) where does consciousness enter into the information processing sequence and (2) how does conscious processing differ from preconscious and unconscious processing. Input analysis is thought to be initially "preconscious," "pre-attentive," fast, involuntary, and automatic. This is followed by "conscious," "focal-attentive" analysis which is relatively slow, voluntary, and flexible. It is thought that simple, familiar stimuli can be identified preconsciously, but conscious processing is needed (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   346 citations  
  • Consciousness from a first-person perspective.Max Velmans - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):702-726.
    This paper replies to the first 36 commentaries on my target article on “Is human information processing conscious?” (Behavioral and Brain Sciences,1991, pp.651-669). The target article focused largely on experimental studies of how consciousness relates to human information processing, tracing their relation from input through to output, while discussion of the implications of the findings both for cognitive psychology and philosophy of mind was relatively brief. The commentaries reversed this emphasis, and so, correspondingly, did the reply. The sequence of topics (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   131 citations  
  • What Is Targeted When We Train Working Memory? Evidence From a Meta-Analysis of the Neural Correlates of Working Memory Training Using Activation Likelihood Estimation.Oshin Vartanian, Vladyslava Replete, Sidney Ann Saint, Quan Lam, Sarah Forbes, Monique E. Beaudoin, Tad T. Brunyé, David J. Bryant, Kathryn A. Feltman, Kristin J. Heaton, Richard A. McKinley, Jan B. F. Van Erp, Annika Vergin & Annalise Whittaker - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13.
    Working memory is the system responsible for maintaining and manipulating information, in the face of ongoing distraction. In turn, WM span is perceived to be an individual-differences construct reflecting the limited capacity of this system. Recently, however, there has been some evidence to suggest that WM capacity can increase through training, raising the possibility that training can functionally alter the neural structures supporting WM. To address the hypothesis that the neural substrates underlying WM are targeted by training, we conducted a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Beyond type 1 vs. type 2 processing: the tri-dimensional way.Alexandra L. Varga & Kai Hamburger - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Reticulo-cortical activity and behavior: A critique of the arousal theory and a new synthesis.C. H. Vanderwolf & T. E. Robinson - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):459-476.
    It is traditionally believed that cerebral activation (the presence of low voltage fast electrical activity in the neocortex and rhythmical slow activity in the hippocampus) is correlated with arousal, while deactivation (the presence of large amplitude irregular slow waves or spindles in both the neocortex and the hippocampus) is correlated with sleep or coma. However, since there are many exceptions, these generalizations have only limited validity. Activated patterns occur in normal sleep (active or paradoxical sleep) and during states of anesthesia (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   172 citations  
  • Has the case been made against the ecumenical view of connectionism?Robert Van Gulick - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):57-58.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Consciousness is not a natural kind.J. van Brakel - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):269-270.
    Blocks distinction between “phenomenal feel” consciousness and “thought/cognition” consciousness is a cultural construction. Consciousness (and its “subspecies”) is not a natural kind. Some crosscultural data are presented to support this.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Brain-behavioral studies: The importance of staying close to the data.C. H. Vanderwolf & T. E. Robinson - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):497-514.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Facilitating Automation in Sentence Processing: The Emergence of Topic and Presupposition in Human Communication.Edoardo Lombardi Vallauri & Viviana Masia - 2018 - Topoi 37 (2):343-354.
    Human attention is limited in its capacity and duration. In language, this is manifested in many ways, but more conspicuously in the strategies by which information is distributed in utterances, that is, their information structures. We contend that the pragmatic categories of Topic and Presupposition precisely meet the necessity to modulate attentional resources on sentence contents, and they do this by “directing” certain contents to automatic and others to controlled processing mechanisms. We discuss experimental findings suggesting that presupposed or topicalized (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Functional dissociations in temporal preparation: Evidence from dual-task performance.Antonino Vallesi, Sandra Arbula & Paolo Bernardis - 2014 - Cognition 130 (2):141-151.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • On models and mechanisms.William R. Uttal - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):459-460.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Facilitation or inhibition from parafoveal words?Geoffrey Underwood - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):48-49.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Attention is necessary for word integration.Geoffrey Underwood - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):698-698.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Attention and awareness: Using the to-be-ignored evidence.Geoffrey Underwood - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):256-256.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Consciousness does not seem to be linked to a single neural mechanism.Carlo Umiltà & Marco Zorzi - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):701-702.
    On the basis of neuropsychological evidence, it is clear that attention should be given a role in any model of consciousness. What is known about the many instances of dissociation between explicit and implicit knowledge after brain damage suggests that conscious experience might not be linked to a restricted area of the brain. Even if it were true that there is a single brain area devoted to consciousness, the subicular area would seem to be an unlikely possibility.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • More on modularity.Carlo Umiltà - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (3):455-456.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sensory adaptation and mismatch negativity.P. Ullsperger & T. Baldeweg - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):255-256.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Note.Joseph Tzelgov - 1997 - Consciousness and Cognition 6 (2-3):441-451.
    The relations between automatic processing and consciousness are discussed in this paper. It is argued that automatic processing should not be identified with the absence of consciousness. The organism has access to representations resulting from automatic processing, but these representations, in contrast to the representations resulting from nonautomatic processing, are not propositional. Therefore monitoring of the process, the defining feature of nonautomatic processing, is not possible.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Some possible implications of Sternberg's triarchic theory of intelligence.Leona E. Tyler - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):301-302.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Blindsight, orgasm, and representational overlap.Michael Tye - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (2):268-269.
    It is argued that there is no fallacy in the reasoning in the example of the thirsty blindsight subject, on one reconstruction of that reasoning. Neither the case of orgasm nor the case of a visual versus an auditory experience as of something overheard shows that phenomenal content is not representational.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Repetition effects with kinesthetic and visual-kinesthetic stimuli.Betty Ann M. Turpin & George E. Stelmach - 1984 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 22 (3):200-202.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Relations among components and processes of memory.Endel Tulving - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):257.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Précis of Elements of episodic memory.Endel Tulving - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):223.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   189 citations  
  • Dynamic-binding theory is not plausible without chaotic oscillation.Ichiro Tsuda - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):475-476.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The unconscious mind: From classical theoretical controversy to controversial contemporary research and a practical illustration of the “error of our ways”.Myron Tsikandilakis, Persefoni Bali, Jan Derrfuss & Peter Chapman - 2019 - Consciousness and Cognition 74:102771.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Individual conscious and unconscious perception of emotion: Theory, methodology and applications.Myron Tsikandilakis, Persefoni Bali, Zhaoliang Yu, Christopher Madan, Jan Derrfuss, Peter Chapman & John Groeger - 2021 - Consciousness and Cognition 94 (C):103172.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Speed and adaptivity in intelligence.Harry C. Triandis - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):301-301.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unified theories and theories that mimic each other's predictions.James T. Townsend - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):458-459.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Should first-order logic be neurally plausible?David S. Touretzky & Scott E. Fahlman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):474-475.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the proper treatment of thermostats.David S. Touretzky - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (1):55-56.
    A set of hypotheses is formulated for a connectionist approach to cognitive modeling. These hypotheses are shown to be incompatible with the hypotheses underlying traditional cognitive models. The connectionist models considered are massively parallel numerical computational systems that are a kind of continuous dynamical system. The numerical variables in the system correspond semantically to fine-grained features below the level of the concepts consciously used to describe the task domain. The level of analysis is intermediate between those of symbolic cognitive models (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Subjective effort derives from a neurological monitor of performance costs and physiological resources.Mattie Tops, Maarten As Boksem & Sander L. Koole - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (6):703-704.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • From retinotopy to recognition: fMRI in human visual cortex.Roger B. H. Tootell, Nouchine K. Hadjikhani, Janine D. Mendola, Sean Marrett & Anders M. Dale - 1998 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2 (5):174-183.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Exploring the Orthogonal Relationship between Controlled and Automated Processes in Skilled Action.John Toner & Aidan Moran - 2020 - Review of Philosophy and Psychology 12 (3):577-593.
    Traditional models of skill learning posit that skilled action unfolds in an automatic manner and that control will prove deleterious to movement and performance proficiency. These perspectives assume that automated processes are characterised by low levels of control and vice versa. By contrast, a number of authors have recently put forward hybrid theories of skilled action which have sought to capture the close integration between fine-grained automatic motor routines and intentional states. Drawing heavily on the work of Bebko et al. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Cognitive-Motor Dual Task Interference Effects on Declarative Memory: A Theory-Based Review.Phillip D. Tomporowski & Ahmed S. Qazi - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 11:524997.
    Bouts of exercise performed either prior to or immediately following study periods enhance encoding and learning. Empirical evidence supporting the benefits of interventions that simultaneously pair physical activity with material to be learned is not conclusive, however. A narrative, theory-based review of dual-task experiments evaluated studies in terms of arousal theories, attention theories, cognitive-energetic theories, and entrainment theories. The pattern of the results of these studies suggests that cognitive-motor interference can either impair or enhance memory of semantic information and the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On giving a more active and selective role to consciousness.Frederick Toates - 1995 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (4):700-701.
    An active role for conscious processes in the production of behaviour is proposed, involving top level controls in a hierarchy of behavioural control. It is suggested that by inhibiting or sensitizing lower levels in the hierarchy conscious processes can play a role in the organization of ongoing behaviour. Conscious control can be more or less evident, according to prevailing circumstances.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Early or late selection? Still an open issue.Steven P. Tipper - 1990 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 (2):255-255.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Just how does ecphory work?Guy Tiberghien - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2):255.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Temporal synchrony and the speed of visual processing.Simon J. Thorpe - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):473-474.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive Predictors of Precautionary Behavior During the COVID-19 Pandemic.Volker Thoma, Leonardo Weiss-Cohen, Petra Filkuková & Peter Ayton - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12:589800.
    The attempts to mitigate the unprecedented health, economic, and social disruptions caused by the COVID-19 pandemic are largely dependent on establishing compliance to behavioral guidelines and rules that reduce the risk of infection. Here, by conducting an online survey that tested participants’ knowledge about the disease and measured demographic, attitudinal, and cognitive variables, we identify predictors of self-reported social distancing and hygiene behavior. To investigate the cognitive processes underlying health-prevention behavior in the pandemic, we co-opted the dual-process model of thinking (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Are infants human?H. S. Terrace - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):425-426.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark