Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Psycho-neural Identity as the Basis for Empirical Research and Theorization in Psychology: An Interview with Mario A. Bunge.Javier Virues-Ortega, Camilo Hurtado-Parrado, Toby L. Martin & Flávia Julio - 2012 - Science & Education 21 (10):1527-1534.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A genealogical map of the concept of habit.Xabier E. Barandiaran & Ezequiel A. Di Paolo - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8 (522):1--7.
    The notion of information processing has dominated the study of the mind for over six decades. However, before the advent of cognitivism, one of the most prominent theoretical ideas was that of Habit. This is a concept with a rich and complex history, which is again starting to awaken interest, following recent embodied, enactive critiques of computationalist frameworks. We offer here a very brief history of the concept of habit in the form of a genealogical network-map. This serves to provide (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • Discussing learning: The quandary of substance.Jack P. Hailman - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):146-146.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Expectancy: The endogenous source of anticipatory activities, including “pseudoconditioned” responses.Patrick J. Sheafor - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):387-389.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • How to change Behavior?Iver H. Iversen - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):457.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The pros and cons of having a word for it.S. F. Walker - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (1):156-157.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Neuropsychology vis-à-vis Skinner's behaviouristic psychology.Gerhard D. Wassermann - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):700-701.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):669-677.
    Responses are strengthened by consequences having to do with the survival of individuals and species. With respect to the provenance of behavior, we know more about ontogenic than phylogenic contingencies. The contingencies responsible for unlearned behavior acted long ago. This remoteness affects our scientific methods, both experimental and conceptual. Until we have identified he variables responsible for an event, we tend to invent causes. Explanatory entities such as “instincts,” “drives,” and “traits” still survive. Unable to show how organisms can behave (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The operant behaviorism of B. F. Skinner.A. Charles Catania - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):473.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Selection by consequences.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):477-481.
    Human behavior is the joint product of (i) contingencies of survival responsible for natural selection, and (ii) contingencies of reinforcement responsible for the repertoires of individuals, including (iii) the special contingencies maintained by an evolved social environment. Selection by consequences is a causal mode found only in living things, or in machines made by living things. It was first recognized in natural selection: Reproduction, a first consequence, led to the evolution of cells, organs, and organisms reproducing themselves under increasingly diverse (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   69 citations  
  • Précis of Behaviorism: A conceptual reconstruction.G. E. Zuriff - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):687-699.
    The conceptual framework of behaviorism is reconstructed in a logical scheme rather than along chronological lines. The resulting reconstruction is faithful to the history of behaviorism and yet meets the contemporary challenges arising from cognitive science, psycholinguistics, and philosophy. In this reconstruction, the fundamental premise is that psychology is to be a natural science, and the major corollaries are that psychology is to be objective and empirical. To a great extent, the reconstruction of behaviorism is an elaboration of behaviorist views (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Pain and parallel processing.Ronald Melzack - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):67-68.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Genetic aspects to differences in foraging behavior.Marla B. Sokolowski - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):348-349.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Reaching for an integrated science of behavior.Clifton Lee Gass - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):337-337.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Pain and behavior.Howard Rachlin - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):43-83.
    There seem to be two kinds of pain: fundamental pain, the intensity of which is a direct function of the intensity of various pain stimuli, and pain, the intensity of which is highly modifiable by such factors as hypnotism, placebos, and the sociocultural setting in which the stimulus occurs.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   209 citations  
  • (2 other versions)The operational analysis of psychological terms.B. F. Skinner - 1945 - Psychological Review 52 (4):270-78.
    The major contributions of operationism have been negative, largely because operationists failed to distinguish logical theories of reference from empirical accounts of language. Behaviorism never finished an adequate formulation of verbal reports and therefore could not convincingly embrace subjective terms. But verbal responses to private stimuli can arise as social products through the contingencies of reinforcement arranged by verbal communities.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   105 citations  
  • On usefulness of the useless: Philosophy as the consciousness of scientific knowledge.Carolina Laurenti, Carlos Eduardo Lopes & Jose Antonio Damasio Abib - 2020 - Behavior and Philosophy 48:91-108.
    This essay explores some possibilities brought by the question about philosophy’s utility for science. We point to some arguments in favor of the importance of philosophy for science in general and Behavior Analysis in particular. We argue that philosophy is the consciousness of science. Without philosophical consciousness, science incurs epistemological naiveties; it uncritically defends scientific neutrality; it risks turning into a mere technique in the service of ideologies that endangers science’s existence. As the philosophy of Behavior Analysis, Radical Behaviorism can (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Contrasting approaches to a theory of learning.Timothy D. Johnston - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):125-139.
    The general process view of learning, which guided research into learning for the first half of this century, has come under attack in recent years from several quarters. One form of criticism has come from proponents of the so-called biological boundaries approach to learning. These theorists have presented a variety of data showing that supposedly general laws of learning may in fact be limited in their applicability to different species and learning tasks, and they argue that the limitations are drawn (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   287 citations  
  • Mind the brain.Martha Wilson - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):393-393.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Memory: A matter of fitness.Juan D. Delius - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):375-376.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What is Weber's Law?R. J. Watt - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):313-314.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Searching for models.Karel Kranda - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):299-300.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sensory analysis of vision.J. J. Kulikowski - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):300-301.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Pavlovian contingencies and conditioned reinforcement.John A. Nevin - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):711.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Is evolution of behavior operant conditioning writ large?Anatol Rapoport - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):696-696.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • B. F. Skinner's operationism.Jon D. Ringen - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):567.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Significance of localized rhythmic activities occurring during the waking state.A. Rougeul, J. J. Bouyer & P. Buser - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (3):488-488.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Choice, optimal foraging, and the delay-reduction hypothesis.Edmund Fantino & Nureya Abarca - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (2):315-330.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   233 citations  
  • Macphail (1987) Revisited: Pigeons Have Much Cognitive Behavior in Common With Humans.Thomas R. Zentall - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 11.
    The hypothesis proposed by Macphail is that differences in intelligent behavior thought to distinguish different species were likely attributed to differences in the context of the tasks being used. Once one corrects for differences in sensory input, motor output, and incentive, it is likely that all vertebrate animals have comparable intellectual abilities. In the present article I suggest a number of tests of this hypothesis with pigeons. In each case, the evidence suggests that either there is evidence for the cognitive (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Exploring Behaviorism : A Networked (Re)integration.Fabian Gunnars - 2020 - In Maarten De Laat, Thomas Ryberg, Nina Bonderup Dohn, Stig Børsen Hansen & Jens Jørgen Hansen (eds.), Networked Learning 2020: Proceedings For The Twelfth International Conference On Networked Learning . pp. 312-318.
    Skinner claimed when outlining his behaviorist philosophy, that concepts exist in the world before anyone identifies them, yet that they are inherently linked to social and cultural descriptions and predictions. The perspective has been dismissed by many researchers in the learning sciences as an important but obsolete relic in origin narratives introducing perspectives. In such a narrative, behaviorism is portrayed as incompatible with complex domains related to agency, emotion and engagement. However, researchers have started to consider its benefits when taking (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Missing variables in studies of animal learning.Wally Welker - 1981 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4 (1):161-161.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A la représentation du temps perdu.John C. Marshall - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):382-383.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The meaning of representation in animal memory.H. L. Roitblat - 1982 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5 (3):353-372.
    A representation is a remnant of previous experience that allows that experience to affect later behavior. This paper develops a metatheoretical view of representation and applies it to issues concerning representation in animals. To describe a representational system one must specify the following: thedomainor range of situations in the represented world to which the system applies; thecontentor set of features encoded and preserved by the system; thecodeor transformational rules relating features of the representation to the corresponding features of the represented (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   312 citations  
  • Sensory analysis and behavior theory.John A. Nevin - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (2):307-307.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Response bias in the yoked control procedure.Edward A. Wasserman - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):477.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The ethology of purpose.Richard S. Marken - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):460.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Chimp communication without conditioning.Katherine Nelson - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):461.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ethology, conditioning, and learning.W. M. S. Russell - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):464.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Why contingencies won't go away.A. Charles Catania & Eliot Shimoff - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):450.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Observing and the delay-reduction hypothesis.Edmund Fantino - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (4):707.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Of false dichotomies and larger frames.Jerome H. Barkow - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):680-681.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Skinner's blind eye.H. J. Eysenck - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):686-687.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Is behaviorism vacuous?Stephen P. Stich - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):647.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • B. F. Skinner's confused philosophy of science.Laurence Hitterdale - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):630.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Behaviorism and “the problem of privacy”.William Lyons - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):635.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Waiting for the world to make me talk and tell me what I meant.Richard P. Brinker & Julian Jaynes - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):554.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Replicators, consequences, and displacement activities.Richard Dawkins - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):486-487.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • First-person behaviorism.George Graham - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (4):704-705.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • THIS → is Learning: A learning process made public.Domenic Berducci - 2011 - Pragmatics and Cognition 19 (3):476-506.
    In this paper I attempt to dissolve two confused ideas inherent in scientific studies of learning: That the locus of learning processes lies hidden inside the mind/brain, and also that this putatively hidden phenomenon causes learned actions. I attempt this dissolution through conceptual argument and data analysis, first by contrasting the use of the concept ‘learning’ in ordinary and scientific interaction, followed by a Wittgenstein-inspired conversation analysis of a micro-longitudinal case of learning interaction — a biochemist teaching lab techniques to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Radical behaviorism is a dead end.Jeff Foss - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (1):59-59.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark