- A Higher-Order Theory of Emotional Consciousness.Joseph LeDoux & Richard Brown - 2017 - Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 114 (10):E2016-E2025.details
|
|
What memory is.Stan Klein - 2015 - WIREs Cognitive Science 6 (1):1-38.details
|
|
Retention and transfer of morse code reception skill by novices: part-whole training.Deborah M. Clawson, Alice F. Healy, K. Anders Ericsson & Lyle E. Bourne - 2001 - Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 7 (2):129.details
|
|
Complex declarative learning.Michelene Th Chi & Stellan Ohlsson - 2005 - In K. Holyoak & B. Morrison (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.details
|
|
A psychobiological theory of attachment.Gary W. Kraemer - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):493-511.details
|
|
Emotions of human infants and mothers and development of the brain.Colwyn Trevarthen - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (3):524-525.details
|
|
P300 as the resolution of negative cortical DC shifts.L. Deecke & W. Lang - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):379.details
|
|
Useful ideas for exploiting time to engineer representations.Richard Rohwer - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):471-471.details
|
|
Dynamic-binding theory is not plausible without chaotic oscillation.Ichiro Tsuda - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):475-476.details
|
|
Must we solve the binding problem in neural hardware?James W. Garson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):459-460.details
|
|
Rule acquisition and variable binding: Two sides of the same coin.P. J. Hampson - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):462-462.details
|
|
On the artificial intelligence paradox.Steffen Hölldobler - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):463-464.details
|
|
Distributing structure over time.John E. Hummel & Keith J. Holyoak - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):464-464.details
|
|
Time phases, pointers, rules and embedding.John A. Barnden - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):451-452.details
|
|
Reasoning, learning and neuropsychological plausibility.Joachim Diederich - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):455-456.details
|
|
Locus-pocus.Carlo Semenza - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):80-80.details
|
|
Modularity need not imply locality: Damaged modules can have nonlocal effects.Edgar Zurif & David Swinney - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):89-90.details
|
|
Modularity, interaction and connectionist neuropsychology.Nick Chater - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):66-67.details
|
|
Further advantages of abandoning the locality assumption in face recognition.Jules Davidoff & Bernard Renault - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):68-68.details
|
|
Genes, specificity, and the lexical/functional distinction in language acquisition.Karin Stromswold - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):648-649.details
|
|
Autonomy of syntactic processing and the role of Broca's area.Angela D. Friederici - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):634-635.details
|
|
(1 other version)A unified framework for addiction: Vulnerabilities in the decision process.Adam Johnson A. David Redish, Steve Jensen - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (4):415.details
|
|
The sense of diachronic personal identity.Stan Klein - 2013 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 12 (4):791-811.details
|
|
Uncovering "Cultural Meaning": Problems and Solutions.Todd Jones - 2004 - Behavior and Philosophy 32 (2):247 - 268.details
|
|
The cry for the other: The biocultural womb of human development.James B. Ashbrook - 1994 - Zygon 29 (3):297-314.details
|
|
The epistemology of evidence in cognitive neuroscience.William P. Bechtel - forthcoming - In Robert A. Skipper, Collin Allen, Rachel Ankeny, Carl F. Craver, Lindley Darden, Gregory Mikkelson & Robert C. Richardson (eds.), Philosophy and the Life Sciences: A Reader. MIT Press.details
|
|
The brain's 'new' science: Psychology, neurophysiology, and constraint.Gary Hatfield - 2000 - Philosophy of Science 67 (3):388-404.details
|
|
Psychoneural reduction of the genuinely cognitive: Some accomplished facts.John Bickle - 1995 - Philosophical Psychology 8 (3):265-85.details
|
|
Reconciling reinforcement learning models with behavioral extinction and renewal: Implications for addiction, relapse, and problem gambling.A. David Redish, Steve Jensen, Adam Johnson & Zeb Kurth-Nelson - 2007 - Psychological Review 114 (3):784-805.details
|
|
Conjunctive representations in learning and memory: Principles of cortical and hippocampal function.Randall C. O'Reilly & Jerry W. Rudy - 2001 - Psychological Review 108 (2):311-345.details
|
|
Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: A complementary-learning-systems approach.Kenneth A. Norman & Randall C. O'Reilly - 2003 - Psychological Review 110 (4):611-646.details
|
|
Homing in on consciousness in the nervous system: An action-based synthesis.Ezequiel Morsella, Christine A. Godwin, Tiffany K. Jantz, Stephen C. Krieger & Adam Gazzaley - 2016 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39:1-70.details
|
|
Introduction to “Implicit memory: Multiple perspectives”.Daniel L. Schacter - 1990 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28 (4):338-340.details
|
|
Implicit memory, the serial position effect, and test awareness.John M. Rybash & Joyce L. Osborne - 1991 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 29 (4):327-330.details
|
|
From epistemology to P3-ology.Rolf Verleger - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):399.details
|
|
Probability mismatch and template mismatch: A paradox in P300 amplitude?Albert Kok - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):388.details
|
|
Updating the context of ERP research.Merlin W. Donald - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):381.details
|
|
P300 and the validity of psychophysiological descriptions of behavior.Igor O. Aleksandrov & Natalia E. Maksimova - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):374.details
|
|
Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?Emanuel Donchin & Michael G. H. Coles - 1988 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11 (3):357.details
|
|
Computational and biological constraints in the psychology of reasoning.Mike Oaksford & Mike Malloch - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):468-469.details
|
|
Toward a unified behavioral and brain science.Jerome A. Feldman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):458-458.details
|
|
Could static binding suffice?Paul R. Cooper - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):453-454.details
|
|
No threat to modularity.Yosef Grodzinsky & Uri Hadar - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (1):70-71.details
|
|
Innateness, autonomy, universality, and the neurobiology of regular and irregular inflectional morphology.David Kemmerer - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):639-641.details
|
|
Double dissociation, modularity, and distributed organization.John A. Bullinaria & Nick Chater - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):632-632.details
|
|
How to grow a human.Michael C. Corballis - 1996 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19 (4):632-633.details
|
|
Russell on Mnemic Causation.Sven Bernecker - 2001 - Principia: An International Journal of Epistemology 5 (1-2):149-186.details
|
|
Evolutionary conceptions of adaptation and brain design.Jay Schulkin - 1989 - World Futures 27 (1):1-15.details
|
|
Empathy and Imagination.Nancy Sherman - 1998 - Midwest Studies in Philosophy 22 (1):82-119.details
|
|
(1 other version)A unified framework for addiction: Vulnerabilities in the decision process.A. David Redish, Steve Jensen & Adam Johnson - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (4):415-437.details
|
|