Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. The sociosemiotic commitment.Dirk Geeraerts - 2016 - Cognitive Linguistics 27 (4):527-542.
    Name der Zeitschrift: Cognitive Linguistics Jahrgang: 27 Heft: 4 Seiten: 527-542.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • (1 other version)The Body in the Mind: The Bodily Basis of Meaning, Imagination, and Reason.Mark Johnson - 1987 - The Personalist Forum 5 (1):58-60.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   411 citations  
  • Metaphors We Live By.George Lakoff & Mark Johnson - 1980 - Ethics 93 (3):619-621.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1382 citations  
  • Why Cognitive Linguistics must embrace the social and pragmatic dimensions of language and how it could do so more seriously.Hans-Jörg Schmid - 2016 - Cognitive Linguistics 27 (4):543-557.
    I will argue that the cognitive-linguistic enterprise should step up its efforts to embrace the social and pragmatic dimensions of language. This claim will be derived from a survey of the premises and promise of the cognitive-linguistic approach to the study of language and be defended in more detail on logical and empirical grounds. Key elements of a usage-based emergentist socio-cognitive approach known as Entrenchment-and-Conventionalization Model (Schmid 2014, 2015) will be presented in order to demonstrate how social and pragmatic aspects (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Network analyses of prepositional meaning: Mirroring whose mind—the linguist’s or the language user’s?Dominiek Sandra & Sally Rice - 1995 - Cognitive Linguistics 6 (1):89-130.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The brain's concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge.Vittorio Gallese & George Lakoff - 2007 - Cognitive Neuropsychology 22 (3-4):455-479.
    Concepts are the elementary units of reason and linguistic meaning. They are conventional and relatively stable. As such, they must somehow be the result of neural activity in the brain. The questions are: Where? and How? A common philosophical position is that all concepts—even concepts about action and perception—are symbolic and abstract, and therefore must be implemented outside the brain’s sensory-motor system. We will argue against this position using (1) neuroscientific evidence; (2) results from neural computation; and (3) results about (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   197 citations  
  • Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind.George Lakoff - 1987 - Philosophy and Rhetoric 22 (4):299-302.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1045 citations  
  • Cognitive Linguistics’ seven deadly sins.Ewa Dąbrowska - 2016 - Cognitive Linguistics 27 (4):479-491.
    Name der Zeitschrift: Cognitive Linguistics Jahrgang: 27 Heft: 4 Seiten: 479-491.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • The Invariance Hypothesis: is abstract reason based on image-schemas?George Lakoff - 1990 - Cognitive Linguistics 1 (1):39-74.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   57 citations  
  • Cognitive Grammar and gesture: Points of convergence, advances and challenges.Kasper I. Kok & Alan Cienki - 2016 - Cognitive Linguistics 27 (1):67-100.
    Name der Zeitschrift: Cognitive Linguistics Jahrgang: 27 Heft: 1 Seiten: 67-100.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • What corpus-based Cognitive Linguistics can and cannot expect from neurolinguistics.Alice Blumenthal-Dramé - 2016 - Cognitive Linguistics 27 (4):493-505.
    This paper argues that neurolinguistics has the potential to yield insights that can feed back into corpus-based Cognitive Linguistics. It starts by discussing how far the cognitive realism of probabilistic statements derived from corpus data currently goes. Against this background, it argues that the cognitive realism of usage-based models could be further enhanced through deeper engagement with neurolinguistics, but also highlights a number of common misconceptions about what neurolinguistics can and cannot do for linguistic theorizing.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Typology and the future of Cognitive Linguistics.William Croft - 2016 - Cognitive Linguistics 27 (4):587-602.
    The relationship between typology and Cognitive Linguistics was first posed in the 1980s, in terms of the relationship between Greenbergian universals and the knowledge of the individual speaker. An answer to this question emerges from understanding the role of linguistic variation in language, from occasions of language use to typological diversity. This in turn requires the contribution of discourse analysis, sociolinguistics, and evolutionary historical linguistics as well as typology and Cognitive Linguistics. While Cognitive Linguistics is part of this enterprise, a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Turning back to experience in Cognitive Linguistics via phenomenology.Jordan Zlatev - 2016 - Cognitive Linguistics 27 (4):559-572.
    Name der Zeitschrift: Cognitive Linguistics Jahrgang: 27 Heft: 4 Seiten: 559-572.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Towards cognitively plausible data science in language research.Petar Milin, Dagmar Divjak, Strahinja Dimitrijević & R. Harald Baayen - 2016 - Cognitive Linguistics 27 (4):507-526.
    Over the past 10 years, Cognitive Linguistics has taken a quantitative turn. Yet, concerns have been raised that this preoccupation with quantification and modelling may not bring us any closer to understanding how language works. We show that this objection is unfounded, especially if we rely on modelling techniques based on biologically and psychologically plausible learning algorithms. These make it possible to take a quantitative approach, while generating and testing specific hypotheses that will advance our understanding of how knowledge of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Working toward a synthesis.Ronald W. Langacker - 2016 - Cognitive Linguistics 27 (4):465-477.
    Name der Zeitschrift: Cognitive Linguistics Jahrgang: 27 Heft: 4 Seiten: 465-477.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Conceptual metaphors in gesture.Kawai Chui - 2011 - Cognitive Linguistics 22 (3):437–458.
    This study investigates metaphoric gestures in face-to-face conversation. It is found that gestures of this kind are mainly performed in the central gesture space with noticeable and discernable configurations, providing visible evidence for cross-domain cognitive mappings and the grounding of conceptual metaphors in people's recurrent bodily experiences and in what people habitually do in social and cultural practices. Moreover, whether metaphorical thinking is conveyed by gesture exclusively or along with metaphoric speech, the manual enactment of even conventional metaphors manifests dynamism (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Does historical linguistics need the Cognitive Commitment? Prosodic change in East Slavic.Tore Nesset - 2016 - Cognitive Linguistics 27 (4):573-585.
    Name der Zeitschrift: Cognitive Linguistics Jahrgang: 27 Heft: 4 Seiten: 573-585.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Cognitive Linguistics, gesture studies, and multimodal communication.Alan Cienki - 2016 - Cognitive Linguistics 27 (4):603-618.
    Name der Zeitschrift: Cognitive Linguistics Jahrgang: 27 Heft: 4 Seiten: 603-618.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations