- (1 other version)How to Construct a Minimal Theory of Mind.Stephen A. Butterfill & Ian A. Apperly - 2013 - Mind and Language 28 (5):606-637.details
|
|
Are there signature limits in early theory of mind?Ella Fizke, Stephen A. Butterfill, Lea van de Loo, Eva Reindl & Hannes Rakoczy - 2017 - Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 162:209-224.details
|
|
The two forms of visuo-spatial perspective taking are differently embodied and subserve different spatial prepositions.Klaus Kessler - 2010 - Frontiers in Psychology 1.details
|
|
Unintentional perspective-taking calculates whether something is seen, but not how it is seen.Andrew Surtees, Dana Samson & Ian Apperly - 2016 - Cognition 148 (C):97-105.details
|
|
Task instructions and implicit theory of mind.Dana Schneider, Zoie E. Nott & Paul E. Dux - 2014 - Cognition 133 (1):43-47.details
|
|
Reaction time profiles of adults’ action prediction reveal two mindreading systems.Katheryn Edwards & Jason Low - 2017 - Cognition 160:1-16.details
|
|
Visual perspective taking impairment in children with autistic spectrum disorder.Antonia F. De C. Hamilton, Rachel Brindley & Uta Frith - 2009 - Cognition 113 (1):37-44.details
|
|
Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states?Stephen Andrew Butterfill & Ian A. Apperly - 2009 - Psychological Review 116 (4):953-970.details
|
|
Efficient belief tracking in adults: The role of task instruction, low-level associative processes and dispositional social functioning.Gaëlle Meert, Jessica Wang & Dana Samson - 2017 - Cognition 168 (C):91-98.details
|
|
Executive function is necessary for perspective selection, not Level-1 visual perspective calculation: Evidence from a dual-task study of adults.Adam W. Qureshi, Ian A. Apperly & Dana Samson - 2010 - Cognition 117 (2):230-236.details
|
|
Domain-general contributions to social reasoning: theory of mind and deontic reasoning re-explored.Margaret C. McKinnon & Morris Moscovitch - 2007 - Cognition 102 (2):179-218.details
|
|
Current evidence for automatic Theory of Mind processing in adults.Dana Schneider, Virginia P. Slaughter & Paul E. Dux - 2017 - Cognition 162 (C):27-31.details
|
|
Two sources of evidence on the non-automaticity of true and false belief ascription.Elisa Back & Ian A. Apperly - 2010 - Cognition 115 (1):54-70.details
|
|
Limits on theory of mind use in adults.Boaz Keysar, Shuhong Lin & Dale J. Barr - 2003 - Cognition 89 (1):25-41.details
|
|
Privileged versus shared knowledge about object identity in real-time referential processing.Mindaugas Mozuraitis, Craig G. Chambers & Meredyth Daneman - 2015 - Cognition 142 (C):148-165.details
|
|
Children exhibit different performance patterns in explicit and implicit theory of mind tasks.Nese Oktay-Gür, Alexandra Schulz & Hannes Rakoczy - 2018 - Cognition 173 (C):60-74.details
|
|
More is not always better: The benefits of cognitive limits.Ralph Hertwig & Peter M. Todd - 2003 - Thinking: Psychological Perspectives on Reasoning, Judgment and Decision Making.details
|
|
Do people automatically track others’ beliefs? Evidence from a continuous measure.Robrecht P. R. D. van der Wel, Natalie Sebanz & Guenther Knoblich - 2014 - Cognition 130 (1):128-133.details
|
|
The cost of thinking about false beliefs: Evidence from adults’ performance on a non-inferential theory of mind task.Ian A. Apperly, Elisa Back, Dana Samson & Lisa France - 2008 - Cognition 106 (3):1093-1108.details
|
|
The role of control functions in mentalizing: Dual-task studies of Theory of Mind and executive function.Rebecca Bull, Louise H. Phillips & Claire A. Conway - 2008 - Cognition 107 (2):663-672.details
|
|