Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Elements of a Plan‐Based Theory of Speech Acts.Philip R. Cohen & C. Raymond Perrault - 1979 - Cognitive Science 3 (3):177-212.
    This paper explores the truism that people think about what they say. It proposes that, to satisfy their own goals, people often plan their speech acts to affect their listeners' beliefs, goals, and emotional states. Such language use can be modelled by viewing speech acts as operators in a planning system, thus allowing both physical and speech acts to be integrated into plans. Methodological issues of how speech acts should be defined in a planbased theory are illustrated by defining operators (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   74 citations  
  • Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?Simon Baron-Cohen, Alan M. Leslie & Uta Frith - 1985 - Cognition 21 (1):37–46.
    We use a new model of metarepresentational development to predict a cognitive deficit which could explain a crucial component of the social impairment in childhood autism. One of the manifestations of a basic metarepresentational capacity is a ‘ theory of mind ’. We have reason to believe that autistic children lack such a ‘ theory ’. If this were so, then they would be unable to impute beliefs to others and to predict their behaviour. This hypothesis was tested using Wimmer (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   653 citations  
  • Mindreaders: the cognitive basis of "theory of mind".Ian Apperly - 2011 - New York: Psychology Press.
    Introduction -- Evidence from children -- Evidence form infants and non-human animals -- Evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology -- Evidence from adults -- The cognitive basis of mindreading -- Elaborating and applying the theory.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   90 citations  
  • Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue.Martin J. Pickering & Simon Garrod - 2004 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (2):169-190.
    Traditional mechanistic accounts of language processing derive almost entirely from the study of monologue. Yet, the most natural and basic form of language use is dialogue. As a result, these accounts may only offer limited theories of the mechanisms that underlie language processing in general. We propose a mechanistic account of dialogue, the interactive alignment account, and use it to derive a number of predictions about basic language processes. The account assumes that, in dialogue, the linguistic representations employed by the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   261 citations  
  • Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind.Paul Bloom - 2000 - Cognition 77 (1):25-31.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   115 citations  
  • (6 other versions)Logic and Conversation.H. Paul Grice - 1975 - In Donald Davidson (ed.), The logic of grammar. Encino, Calif.: Dickenson Pub. Co.. pp. 64-75.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1089 citations  
  • Using Language.Herbert H. Clark - 1996 - Cambridge University Press.
    Herbert Clark argues that language use is more than the sum of a speaker speaking and a listener listening. It is the joint action that emerges when speakers and listeners, writers and readers perform their individual actions in coordination, as ensembles. In contrast to work within the cognitive sciences, which has seen language use as an individual process, and to work within the social sciences, which has seen it as a social process, the author argues strongly that language use embodies (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   325 citations  
  • Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?David Premack & Guy Woodruff - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (4):515-526.
    An individual has a theory of mind if he imputes mental states to himself and others. A system of inferences of this kind is properly viewed as a theory because such states are not directly observable, and the system can be used to make predictions about the behavior of others. As to the mental states the chimpanzee may infer, consider those inferred by our own species, for example, purpose or intention, as well as knowledge, belief, thinking, doubt, guessing, pretending, liking, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   926 citations  
  • Metarepresentations: A Multidisciplinary Perspective.Dan Sperber (ed.) - 2000 - Oxford University Press USA.
    This the tenth volume in the Vancouver Studies in Cogntive Science series. It concerns metarepresentation: the construction and use of representations that represent other representations. Metarepresentations are ubiquitous among human beings, whenever we think or talk about mental states or linguistic acts, or theorize about the mind or language. It is crucial to the unconscious process we use to divine the mental states of others, and ultimately to any workable theory of the mind. This volume collects previously unpublished studies on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   46 citations  
  • Common ground and everyday language use: Comments on Horton and Keysar (1996).James W. Polichak & Richard J. Gerrig - 1998 - Cognition 66 (2):183-189.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • (6 other versions)Logic and Conversation.H. Paul Grice - 2013 - In Maite Ezcurdia & Robert J. Stainton (eds.), The Semantics-Pragmatics Boundary in Philosophy. Peterborough, CA: Broadview Press. pp. 47.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   685 citations  
  • Grounding in communication.Herbert H. Clark & Susan E. Brennan - 1991 - In Lauren Resnick, Levine B., M. John, Stephanie Teasley & D. (eds.), Perspectives on Socially Shared Cognition. American Psychological Association. pp. 13--1991.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   76 citations  
  • Metarepresentation.Leda Cosmides, John Tooby & Dan Sperber - 2000 - In Dan Sperber (ed.), Metarepresentations: A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Oxford University Press USA. pp. 53.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Referring as a collaborative process.Herbert H. Clark & Deanna Wilkes-Gibbs - 1986 - Cognition 22 (1):1-39.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   195 citations  
  • Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception.H. Wimmer - 1983 - Cognition 13 (1):103-128.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   825 citations  
  • Beliefs about beliefs [P&W, SR&B].Daniel C. Dennett - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (4):568-570.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   204 citations  
  • When is attribution of beliefs justified? [P&W].Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 1978 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 (4):592-593.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • A theory of memory retrieval.Roger Ratcliff - 1978 - Psychological Review 85 (2):59-108.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   369 citations  
  • A retrieval theory of priming in memory.Roger Ratcliff & Gail McKoon - 1988 - Psychological Review 95 (3):385-408.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  • Contributing to Discourse.Herbert H. Clark & Edward F. Schaefer - 1989 - Cognitive Science 13 (2):259-294.
    For people to contribute to discourse, they must do more than utter the right sentence at the right time. The basic requirement is that they add to their common ground in an orderly way. To do this, we argue, they try to establish for each utterance the mutual belief that the addressees have understood what the speaker meant well enough for current purposes. This is accomplished by the collective actions of the current contributor and his or her partners, and these (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   81 citations  
  • Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access.Stephen D. Goldinger - 1998 - Psychological Review 105 (2):251-279.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   121 citations  
  • "Schema abstraction" in a multiple-trace memory model.Douglas L. Hintzman - 1986 - Psychological Review 93 (4):411-428.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   209 citations  
  • When do speakers take into account common ground?William S. Horton & Boaz Keysar - 1996 - Cognition 59 (1):91-117.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   71 citations  
  • Mutual Knowledge.N. V. Smith & Colloquium on Mutual Knowledge - 1982
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Can theory of mind grow up? Mindreading in adults, and its implications for the development and neuroscience of mindreading.Ian Apperly - 2013 - In Simon Baron-Cohen, Michael Lombardo & Helen Tager-Flusberg (eds.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives From Developmental Social Neuroscience. Oxford University Press. pp. 72.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Pragmatic effects on reference resolution in a collaborative task: evidence from eye movements.Joy E. Hanna & Michael K. Tanenhaus - 2004 - Cognitive Science 28 (1):105-115.
    In order to investigate whether addressees can make immediate use of speaker‐based constraints during reference resolution, participant addressees' eye movements were monitored as they helped a confederate cook follow a recipe. Objects were located in the helper's area, which the cook could not reach, and the cook's area, which both could reach. Critical referring expressions matched one object (helper's area) or two objects (helper's and cook's areas), and were produced when the cook's hands were empty or full, which defined the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   35 citations  
  • Cognitive Status and the Form of Referring Expressions in Discourse.Jeanette Gundel, Nancy Hedberg & Ron Zacharski - 1993 - Language 69:274--307.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   103 citations  
  • A retrieval model for both recognition and recall.Gary Gillund & Richard M. Shiffrin - 1984 - Psychological Review 91 (1):1-67.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   140 citations  
  • (1 other version)False-belief understanding in infants.Renée Baillargeon, Rose M. Scott & Zijing He - 2010 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (3):110–118.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   193 citations  
  • Limits on theory of mind use in adults.Boaz Keysar, Shuhong Lin & Dale J. Barr - 2003 - Cognition 89 (1):25-41.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   113 citations  
  • Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective in Sperber.D. Sperber - 2000 - In Dan Sperber (ed.), Metarepresentations: A Multidisciplinary Perspective. Oxford University Press USA.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • Listeners invest in an assumed other’s perspective despite cognitive cost.Nicholas D. Duran, Rick Dale & Roger J. Kreuz - 2011 - Cognition 121 (1):22-40.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Less Is More: A Minimalist Account of Joint Action in Communication.Hadas Shintel & Boaz Keysar - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (2):260-273.
    Language use can be viewed as a form of joint activity that requires the coordination of meaning between individuals. Because the linguistic signal is notoriously ambiguous, interlocutors need to draw upon additional sources of information to resolve ambiguity and achieve shared understanding. One way individuals can achieve coordination is by using inferences about the interlocutor’s intentions and mental states to adapt their behavior. However, such an inferential process can be demanding in terms of both time and cognitive resources. Here, we (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   23 citations  
  • Do people automatically track others’ beliefs? Evidence from a continuous measure.Robrecht P. R. D. van der Wel, Natalie Sebanz & Guenther Knoblich - 2014 - Cognition 130 (1):128-133.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Revisiting the Memory‐Based Processing Approach to Common Ground.William S. Horton & Richard J. Gerrig - 2016 - Topics in Cognitive Science 8 (4):780-795.
    Horton and Gerrig outlined a memory-based processing model of conversational common ground that provided a description of how speakers could both strategically and automatically gain access to information about others through domain-general memory processes acting over ordinary memory traces. In this article, we revisit this account, reviewing empirical findings that address aspects of this memory-based model. In doing so, we also take the opportunity to clarify what we believe this approach implies about the cognitive psychology of common ground, and just (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Spatial perspective-taking in conversation.Michael F. Schober - 1993 - Cognition 47 (1):1-24.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • Partner‐Specific Adaptation in Dialog.Susan E. Brennan & Joy E. Hanna - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (2):274-291.
    No one denies that people adapt what they say and how they interpret what is said to them, depending on their interactive partners. What is controversial is when and how they do so. Several psycholinguistics research programs have found what appear to be failures to adapt to partners in the early moments of processing and have used this evidence to argue for modularity in the language processing architecture, claiming that the system cannot take into account a partner’s distinct needs or (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • The impact of memory demands on audience design during language production.William S. Horton & Richard J. Gerrig - 2005 - Cognition 96 (2):127-142.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   44 citations  
  • (1 other version)False-belief understanding in infants.Zijing He Renée Baillargeon, Rose M. Scott - 2010 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (3):110.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   146 citations  
  • Pragmatic expectations and linguistic evidence: Listeners anticipate but do not integrate common ground.Dale J. Barr - 2008 - Cognition 109 (1):18-40.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination.Simon Garrod & Anthony Anderson - 1987 - Cognition 27 (2):181-218.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   126 citations