Switch to: Citations

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Toward a statistical theory of learning.William K. Estes - 1950 - Psychological Review 57 (2):94-107.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   104 citations  
  • Changes in auditory frequency guide visual–spatial attention.Julia A. Mossbridge, Marcia Grabowecky & Satoru Suzuki - 2011 - Cognition 121 (1):133-139.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect.E. Rusconi, B. Kwan, B. Giordano, C. Umilta & B. Butterworth - 2006 - Cognition 99 (2):113-129.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   49 citations  
  • Sounds exaggerate visual shape.Timothy D. Sweeny, Emmanuel Guzman-Martinez, Laura Ortega, Marcia Grabowecky & Satoru Suzuki - 2012 - Cognition 124 (2):194-200.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The Reverse Hierarchy Theory of Visual Perceptual Learning.Merav Ahissar & Shaul Hochstein - 2004 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8 (10):457-464.
    Perceptual learning can be defined as practice-induced improvement in the ability to perform specific perceptual tasks. We previously proposed the Reverse Hierarchy Theory as a unifying concept that links behavioral findings of visual learning with physiological and anatomical data. Essentially, it asserts that learning is a top-down guided process, which begins at high-level areas of the visual system, and when these do not suffice, progresses backwards to the input levels, which have a better signal-to-noise ratio. This simple concept has proved (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • Effects of some variations in auditory input upon visual choice reaction time.Ira H. Bernstein & Barry A. Edelstein - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 87 (2):241.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy.Edward Awh, Artem V. Belopolsky & Jan Theeuwes - 2012 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (8):437.
    Prominent models of attentional control assert a dichotomy between top-down and bottom-up control, with the former determined by current selection goals and the latter determined by physical salience. This theoretical dichotomy, however, fails to explain a growing number of cases in which neither current goals nor physical salience can account for strong selection biases. For example, equally salient stimuli associated with reward can capture attention, even when this contradicts current selection goals. Thus, although 'top-down' sources of bias are sometimes defined (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   95 citations  
  • Out of sight, out of mind: The attentional blink can eliminate synaesthetic colours.Anina N. Rich & Jason B. Mattingley - 2010 - Cognition 114 (3):320-328.
    Mechanisms of selective attention exert a powerful influence on visual perception. We examined whether attentional selection is necessary for generation of the vivid colours experienced by individuals with grapheme-colour synaesthesia. Twelve synaesthetes and matched controls viewed rapid serial displays of nonsense characters within which were embedded an oriented grating (T1) and a letter-prime (T2), forming a modified attentional blink (AB) task. At the end of the stream a coloured probe appeared that was either congruent or incongruent with the synaesthetic colour (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Perceptual load influences auditory space perception in the ventriloquist aftereffect.Ranmalee Eramudugolla, Marc R. Kamke, Salvador Soto-Faraco & Jason B. Mattingley - 2011 - Cognition 118 (1):62-74.
    A period of exposure to trains of simultaneous but spatially offset auditory and visual stimuli can induce a temporary shift in the perception of sound location. This phenomenon, known as the 'ventriloquist aftereffect', reflects a realignment of auditory and visual spatial representations such that they approach perceptual alignment despite their physical spatial discordance. Such dynamic changes to sensory representations are likely to underlie the brain's ability to accommodate inter-sensory discordance produced by sensory errors (particularly in sound localization) and variability in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity.V. Walsh - 2003 - Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (11):483-488.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   191 citations  
  • On differentiation: A case study of the development of the concepts of size, weight, and density.Carol Smith, Susan Carey & Marianne Wiser - 1985 - Cognition 21 (3):177-237.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   109 citations  
  • A systematic, large-scale study of synaesthesia: implications for the role of early experience in lexical-colour associations.Anina N. Rich, John L. Bradshaw & Jason B. Mattingley - 2005 - Cognition 98 (1):53-84.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   45 citations  
  • Measuring Individual Differences in Implicit Cognition: The Implicit Association Test.Debbie E. McGhee, Jordan L. K. Schwartz & Anthony G. Greenwald - 1998 - Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 (6):1464-1480.
    An implicit association test (IAT) measures differential association of 2 target concepts with an attribute. The 2 concepts appear in a 2-choice task (e.g., flower vs. insect names), and the attribute in a 2nd task (e.g., pleasant vs. unpleasant words for an evaluation attribute). When instructions oblige highly associated categories (e.g., flower + pleasant) to share a response key, performance is faster than when less associated categories (e.g., insect + pleasant) share a key. This performance difference implicitly measures differential association (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   275 citations  
  • Is the exogenous orienting of spatial attention truly automatic? Evidence from unimodal and multisensory studies.Valerio Santangelo & Charles Spence - 2008 - Consciousness and Cognition 17 (3):989-1015.
    The last decade has seen great progress in the study of the nature of crossmodal links in exogenous and endogenous spatial attention . Exogenous spatial cuing studies of human crossmodal attention and multisensory integration. In C. Spence, & J. Driver , Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention . Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.], for a recent review). A growing body of research now highlights the existence of robust crossmodal links between auditory, visual, and tactile spatial attention. However, until recently, studies of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition.John A. Bargh - 1994 - In Robert S. Wyer & Thomas K. Srull (eds.), Handbook of Social Cognition: Applications. Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   90 citations  
  • Synesthesia: Implications for attention, binding, and consciousness--a commentary.Anne Treisman - 2005 - In Robertson, C. L. & N. Sagiv (eds.), Synesthesia: Perspectives From Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford University Press. pp. 239-254.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Specifying the relations between automaticity and consciousness: A theoretical note.Joseph Tzelgov - 1997 - Consciousness and Cognition 6:441-51.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • Neonatal synesthesia: A re-evaluation.D. Maurer & C. Mondloch - 2005 - In Robertson, C. L. & N. Sagiv (eds.), Synesthesia: Perspectives From Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford University Press.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • A mathematical model for simple learning.Robert R. Bush & Frederick Mosteller - 1951 - Psychological Review 58 (5):313-323.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • Exogenous spatial cuing studies of human crossmodal attention and multisensory integration.Charles Spence, John Mcdonald & Jon Driver - 2004 - In Charles Spence & Jon Driver (eds.), Crossmodal Space and Crossmodal Attention. Oxford University Press.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • Hearing colors, tasting shapes.Vilayanur S. Ramachandran & Edward M. Hubbard - 2003 - Scientific American (May):52-59.
    Jones and Coleman are among a handful of otherwise normal as a child and the number 5 was red and 6 was green. This the- people who have synesthesia. They experience the ordinary ory does not answer why only some people retain such vivid world in extraordinary ways and seem to inhabit a mysterious sensory memories, however. You might _think _of cold when you no-man’s-land between fantasy and reality. For them the sens- look at a picture of an ice cube, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • Modularity and relevance: How can a massively modular mind be flexible and context-sensitive.Dan Sperber - 2005 - In Peter Carruthers, Stephen Laurence & Stephen P. Stich (eds.), The Innate Mind: Structure and Contents. New York, US: Oxford University Press USA. pp. 53.
    The claim that the human cognitive system tends to allocate resources to the processing of available inputs according to their expected relevance is at the basis of relevance theory. The main thesis of this chapter is that this allocation can be achieved without computing expected relevance. When an input meets the input condition of a given modular procedure, it gives this procedure some initial level of activation. Input-activated procedures are in competition for the energy resources that would allow them to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • Modularity and relevance: How can a massively modular mind be flexible and context-sensitive.Dan Sperber - 2005 - In Peter Carruthers, Stephen Laurence & Stephen P. Stich (eds.), The Innate Mind: Structure and Contents. New York, US: Oxford University Press USA. pp. 53.
    The claim that the human cognitive system tends to allocate resources to the processing of available inputs according to their expected relevance is at the basis of relevance theory. The main thesis of this chapter is that this allocation can be achieved without computing expected relevance. When an input meets the input condition of a given modular procedure, it gives this procedure some initial level of activation. Input-activated procedures are in competition for the energy resources that would allow them to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The shape of words in the brain.Vanja Kovic, Kim Plunkett & Gert Westermann - 2010 - Cognition 114 (1):19-28.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  • The spatial character of high and low tones.C. C. Pratt - 1930 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 13 (3):278.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Does binding of synesthetic color to the evoking grapheme require attention?Noam Sagiv, Jeffrey Heer & Lynn Robertson - 2006 - Cortex 42 (2):232-42.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations