Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. (1 other version)Reimagining Illocutionary Force.Lucy McDonald - 2022 - Philosophical Quarterly 72 (4):918-939.
    Speech act theorists tend to hold that the illocutionary force of an utterance is determined by one interlocutor alone: either the speaker or the hearer. Yet experience tells us that the force of our utterances is not determined unilaterally. Rather, communication often feels collaborative. In this paper, I develop and defend a collaborative theory of illocutionary force, according to which the illocutionary force of an utterance is determined by an agreement reached by the speaker and the hearer. This theory, which (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • (1 other version)Reimagining Illocutionary Force.Lucy McDonald - forthcoming - The Philosophical Quarterly.
    Speech act theorists tend to hold that the illocutionary force of an utterance is determined by one interlocutor alone: either the speaker or the hearer. Yet experience tells us that the force of our utterances is not determined unilaterally. Rather, communication often feels collaborative. In this paper, I develop and defend a collaborative theory of illocutionary force, according to which the illocutionary force of an utterance is determined by an agreement reached by the speaker and the hearer. This theory, which (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • (Mis)communication through stickers in online group discussions: A multiple-case study.Qian Chen, Susan C. Herring, Khe Foon Hew & Ying Tang - 2021 - Discourse and Communication 15 (5):582-606.
    Sticker use is an increasingly popular part of daily messaging activity. However, little is known regarding the types, functions, and outcomes of sticker use in authentic online communications. To investigate these phenomena, we analysed sticker use in five small mobile-messaging-facilitated discussion groups initiated by students for course projects in an Asian university. The students used four types of stickers, among which ‘animated picture without text’ was the most frequent. Sticker functions fell into two main categories: as a tone indicator with (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Role of Eye Gaze in Regulating Turn Taking in Conversations: A Systematized Review of Methods and Findings.Ziedune Degutyte & Arlene Astell - 2021 - Frontiers in Psychology 12.
    Eye gaze plays an important role in communication but understanding of its actual function or functions and the methods used to elucidate this have varied considerably. This systematized review was undertaken to summarize both the proposed functions of eye gaze in conversations of healthy adults and the methodological approaches employed. The eligibility criteria were restricted to a healthy adult population and excluded studies that manipulated eye gaze behavior. A total of 29 articles—quantitative, qualitative and mixed methods were returned, with a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • RoboDoc: Semiotic resources for achieving face-to-screenface formation with a telepresence robot.Brian L. Due - 2021 - Semiotica 2021 (238):253-278.
    Face-to-face interaction is a primordial site for human activity and intersubjectivity. Empirical studies have shown how people reflexively exhibit a face orientation and work to establish a formation in which everyone is facing each other in local participation frameworks. The Face has also been described by, e.g., Levinas as the basis for a first ethical philosophy. Humans have established these Face-formations when interacting since time immemorial, but what happens when one of the participants is present through a telepresence robot? Based (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Explorations in engagement for humans and robots.Candace L. Sidner, Christopher Lee, Cory D. Kidd, Neal Lesh & Charles Rich - 2005 - Artificial Intelligence 166 (1-2):140-164.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Positive politeness as discourse process: politeness practices of high-functioning children with autism and Asperger Syndrome.Karen Gainer Sirota - 2004 - Discourse Studies 6 (2):229-251.
    This study draws upon naturalistic ethnographic data to expand current understandings regarding the socio-communicative capabilities and challenges of children with autism spectrum disorders in mid-childhood. Affording a view of the children’s spontaneous interactions within naturally occurring family and community settings, the study explores a range of discursive resources utilized by the children to accomplish socially reciprocal positive politeness practices in tandem with others. Emphasizing contextualized deployment of politeness forms in interaction, the practice-based conception developed here construes positive politeness as a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Autism and the Social World: An Anthropological Perspective.Olga Solomon, Karen Gainer Sirota, Tamar Kremer-Sadlik & Elinor Ochs - 2004 - Discourse Studies 6 (2):147-183.
    This article offers an anthropological perspective on autism, a condition at once neurological and social, which complements existing psychological accounts of the disorder, expanding the scope of inquiry from the interpersonal domain, in which autism has been predominantly examined, to the socio-cultural one. Persons with autism need to be viewed not only as individuals in relation to other individuals, but as members of social groups and communities who act, displaying both social competencies and difficulties, in relation to socially and culturally (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Preference organization of sequence-initiating actions: The case of explicit account solicitations.Galina B. Bolden & Jeffrey D. Robinson - 2010 - Discourse Studies 12 (4):501-533.
    This article extends prior conversation analytic research on the preference organization of sequence-initiating actions. Across two languages, this article examines one such action: explicitly soliciting an account for human conduct. Prior work demonstrates that this action conveys a challenging stance towards the warrantability of the accountable event/conduct. When addressees are somehow responsible for the accountable event/conduct, explicit solicitations of accounts are frequently critical of, and thus embody disaffiliation with, addressees. This article demonstrates that, when explicit solicitations of accounts embody disaffiliation, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  • Contingency and Units in Interaction.Cecilia E. Ford - 2004 - Discourse Studies 6 (1):27-52.
    Starting with Houtkoop and Mazeland’s study of discourse units, and touching upon recent studies aimed at detailing unit projection in interaction, this article argues that the drive toward abstract and discrete models for units and unit projection is potentially misleading. While it has been established that to engage in talk-in-interaction, as it unfolds in real time, participants rely on projectable units, research aimed at defining units unintentionally backgrounds the contingency inherent in interaction. A central function of language for collaborative action (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Responding to the Double Implication of Telemarketers’ Opinion Queries.Harrie Mazeland - 2004 - Discourse Studies 6 (1):95-115.
    During a call, telemarketers sometimes solicit respondent’s opinions about a product or service. This turns out to be a query with multiple implications, and respondents are alive to them. On the one hand, the recipient orients to a local preference to evaluate the telemarketer’s product positively. On the other hand, a positive assessment may result in expectations and commitments that survive the sequence and that are relevant for the call’s outcome. The recipient is faced with two types of preference structures, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Agreement, acknowledgment, and alignment: The discourse-pragmatic functions of hao and dui in Taiwan Mandarin conversation.Meng-Ying Lin, David Goodman, Pi-Hua Tsai & Yu-Fang Wang - 2010 - Discourse Studies 12 (2):241-267.
    This study draws on Relevance Theory, Conversation Analysis, and Politeness Theory in investigating a full range of discourse functions for hao and dui with reference to recurrent patterns, distributions, and forms of organization in a large corpus of talk. Special emphasis is placed on a comparison of hao and dui in combination with a small subset of discourse particles: in particular hao/hao le/ hao la/hao a/hao ba and dui/dui a/dui le in spoken discourse. We find that both of the markers (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Aligning in and through interaction: Children getting in and out of spontaneous activity.Susan Danby, Charlotte Cobb-Moore & Johanna Rendle-Short - 2014 - Discourse Studies 16 (6):792-815.
    Spontaneous play, important for forming the basis of friendships and peer relations, is a complex activity involving the management and production of talk-in-interaction. This article focuses on the intricacies of social interaction, emphasizing the link between alignment and affiliation, and the range and importance of verbal and nonverbal interactive devices available to children. Analysis of the way in which two girls, one of whom has been diagnosed with Asperger’s Syndrome, engage in spontaneous activities demonstrates the potential for interactional difficulty due (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Word repeats as unit ends.Emanuel A. Schegloff - 2011 - Discourse Studies 13 (3):367-380.
    Turns-at-talk are fundamental units of participation in talk-in-interaction, and turn-constructional-units are the basic building blocks for turns. Possible completion of a TCU is, in principle, the possible completion of the turn, but multi-unit turns are not uncommon, and participants have practices for constructing multi-unit turns and for recognizing them in the course of their production. This article offers an account of one practice usable by speakers and recipients to convey and recognize the designed completion of a multi-TCU turn and/or a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • School Marks and Teachers' Accountability to Colleagues.Maykel Verkuyten - 2000 - Discourse Studies 2 (4):452-472.
    Educational assessment and marking, in particular, give teachers considerable power over students and their future possibilities. Analysing a teachers' meeting discussing school reports in a secondary school showed that marking involves teachers' accountability to colleagues. An examination of the situated interpretations and explanations of unsatisfactory school marks also showed how various discursive devices were used for building a factual account. Confirmed expectations, extreme case formulations, introducing corroborating witnesses, the deployment of cited others, defining exceptional cases, detailed descriptions and narratives, and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Doing reflecting: Embodied solitary confirmation of instructed enactment.Yusuke Arano - 2020 - Discourse Studies 22 (3):261-290.
    Employing conversation analysis, this article investigates instructions in lessons for guitar and Japanese calligraphy. In receiving an instruction, the students in the guitar lessons are expected to immediately follow the instruction. In contrast to the guitar lessons, the students in the calligraphy lessons are not institutionally expected to immediately follow the teacher’s instructions but to receive them. However, the students often gesticulate what they learned from and could make sense of the prior instruction upon completion of an instruction activity. I (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Scripted communication for service standardisation? What analysis of conversation can tell us about the fast-food service encounter.Uma Chandra-Sagaran & Mei Yuit Chan - 2019 - Discourse and Communication 13 (1):3-25.
    In highly routinised service encounter interactions, communication is often guided by service scripts that are the material embodiment of institutional expectations of how the service interaction is to be conducted. However, counter to common belief that scripted communication is well-controlled and homogeneous in its execution, observation of actual talk reveals interesting patterns and variations that reflect the ways in which participants make meaning of and perform their respective roles within the interaction towards achieving the overall goal of the service communication. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • (1 other version)“Talk to you later” : Doing social robotics with conversation analysis. Towards the development of an automatic system for the prediction of disengagement.Nicolas Rollet & Chloé Clavel - 2020 - Interaction Studies 21 (2):268-292.
    This article presents an applied discussion of the possibility of integrating conversation analysis (CA) methodology into that of machine learning. The aim is to improve the detection of that which resembles disengagement in the interaction between a robot and a human. We offer a novel analytical assemblage at the heart of the two disciplines, and namely on the level of the annotation schemes provided by conversation analysis transcription methods. First, we demonstrate that the need for a stable structure in establishing (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Italien et sarde dans l’espace urbain de Cagliari : contact de langues et contexte d’interaction.Giovanni Depau - 2012 - Corela. Cognition, Représentation, Langage 11 (HS).
    Cet article vise à fournir des éléments de réflexion sur le rôle du contexte dans un cadre de production bilingue à travers la présentation d’interactions enregistrées dans un milieu urbain. La Sardaigne constitue notre domaine d’enquête spécifique. Cette étude se fonde sur l’analyse d’un corpus oral consistant d’un ensemble de textes bilingues « sarde-italien » recueillis dans des contextes interactionnels assez variés et se caractérisant par divers degrés de formalité. L’analyse effectuée dans notre étude est centrée principalement sur une démarche (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Illocutionary Frustration.Samia Hesni - 2018 - Mind 127 (508):947-976.
    This paper proposes a new category of linguistic harm: that of illocutionary frustration. I argue against Jennifer Hornsby and Rae Langton’s notion of illocutionary silencing by challenging their claim that silencing occurs when there is a lack of uptake of the speaker’s illocutionary act. I look at two scenarios that their view treats differently and argue that these scenarios warrant the same kind of analysis; Hornsby and Langton’s notion of silencing can’t capture the purported difference they want it to capture. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Le live-tweet d’audience de procès : forme numérique d’un genre journalistique de tenant lieu.Elżbieta Biardzka - 2024 - Corela. Cognition, Représentation, Langage HS-41 (HS-41).
    We are interested in the study of tweets from trial hearings (LTAP) which is a new digital form of the online journalistic genre: a cross over between judicial chronicles and reports. Supported by new technologies, LTAP serves as an intermediary between judiciary and general public. The LTAP falls into a generic subset of discourse which could be called a “discourse substitute”, since it's a discourse representing another discourse. Therefore, LTAP is thus constitutively a part of the representation of the other (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The Practicalities of Menu Use: Improvisation in Screen-Based Activity.P. Luff & C. Heath - 1993 - Journal of Intelligent Systems 3 (2-4):251-296.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ghost-in-the-Machine reveals human social signals for human–robot interaction.Sebastian Loth, Katharina Jettka, Manuel Giuliani & Jan P. de Ruiter - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    We used a new method called “Ghost-in-the-Machine” (GiM) to investigate social interactions with a robotic bartender taking orders for drinks and serving them. Using the GiM paradigm allowed us to identify how human participants recognize the intentions of customers on the basis of the output of the robotic recognizers. Specifically, we measured which recognizer modalities (e.g., speech, the distance to the bar) were relevant at different stages of the interaction. This provided insights into human social behavior necessary for the development (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • A Simplest Systematics for the Organization of Turn-Taking for Conversation.Gail Jefferson, Andrei Korbut, Harvey Sacks & Emmanuel Schegloff - 2015 - Russian Sociological Review 14 (1):142-202.
    The article is the first Russian translation of the most well-known piece in conversation analysis, written by the founders of CA Harvey Sacks, Emanuel Schegloff and Gail Jefferson. It has become a milestone in the development of the discipline. The authors offer a comprehensive approach to the study of conversational interactions. The approach is based on the analysis of detailed transcripts of the records of natural conversations. The authors show that in the course of the conversation co-conversationalists use a number (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   366 citations  
  • Enrolling the Citizen in Sustainability: Membership Categorization, Morality and Civic Participation.Jennifer Summerville & Barbara Adkins - 2007 - Human Studies 30 (4):429-446.
    This article examines the common-sense and methodical ways in which “the citizen” is produced and enrolled as an active participant in “sustainable” regional planning. Using Membership Categorization Analysis, we explicate how the categorization procedures in the Foreword of a draft regional planning policy interactionally produce the identity of “the citizen” and “civic values and obligations” in relation to geographic place and institutional categories. Furthermore, we show how positioning practices establish a relationship between authors (government) and readers (citizens) where both are (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Getting of Sexuality: Foucault, Garfinkel and the Analysis of Sexual Discourse.Alec McHoul - 1986 - Theory, Culture and Society 3 (2):65-79.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Referring as a collaborative process.Herbert H. Clark & Deanna Wilkes-Gibbs - 1986 - Cognition 22 (1):1-39.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   195 citations  
  • Talking and driving: Multiactivity in the car.Lorenza Mondada - 2012 - Semiotica 2012 (191).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Navigating joint projects with dialogue.Adrian Bangerter & Herbert H. Clark - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (2):195-225.
    Dialogue has its origins in joint activities, which it serves to coordinate. Joint activities, in turn, usually emerge in hierarchically nested projects and subprojects. We propose that participants use dialogue to coordinate two kinds of transitions in these joint projects: vertical transitions, or entering and exiting joint projects; and horizontal transitions, or continuing within joint projects. The participants help signal these transitions with project markers, words such as uh-huh, m-hm, yeah, okay, or all right. These words have been studied mainly (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   21 citations  
  • Real‐Time Investigation of Referential Domains in Unscripted Conversation: A Targeted Language Game Approach.Sarah Brown-Schmidt & Michael K. Tanenhaus - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (4):643-684.
    Two experiments examined the restriction of referential domains during unscripted conversation by analyzing the modification and online interpretation of referring expressions. Experiment 1 demonstrated that from the earliest moments of processing, addressees interpreted referring expressions with respect to referential domains constrained by the conversation. Analysis of eye movements during the conversation showed elimination of standard competition effects seen with scripted language. Results from Experiment 2 pinpointed two pragmatic factors responsible for restriction of the referential domains used by speakers to design (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • Rhetoric and Dialectic from the Standpoint of Normative Pragmatics.Scott Jacobs - 2000 - Argumentation 14 (3):261-286.
    Normative pragmatics can bridge the differences between dialectical and rhetorical theories in a way that saves the central insights of both. Normative pragmatics calls attention to how the manifest strategic design of a message produces interpretive effects and interactional consequences. Argumentative analysis of messages should begin with the manifest persuasive rationale they communicate. But not all persuasive inducements should be treated as arguments. Arguments express with a special pragmatic force propositions where those propositions stand in particular inferential relations to one (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   63 citations  
  • “Questions” in Argument Sequences in Japanese.Tomoyo Takagi - 1999 - Human Studies 22 (2/4):397 - 423.
    The present study reports on the use of a linguistic category "interrogative," which has been traditionally associated with the act of questioning, and its use in argument talk in Japanese. Based on the observation that interrogative utterances in argument data are regularly followed by non-answers, it is argued that interrogative utterances in argument sequences may not be designed/interpreted as doing questioning. Such use of interrogatives can become an orderly practice to which participants orient themselves in social activities recognizable as arguments. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue.Martin J. Pickering & Simon Garrod - 2004 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (2):169-190.
    Traditional mechanistic accounts of language processing derive almost entirely from the study of monologue. Yet, the most natural and basic form of language use is dialogue. As a result, these accounts may only offer limited theories of the mechanisms that underlie language processing in general. We propose a mechanistic account of dialogue, the interactive alignment account, and use it to derive a number of predictions about basic language processes. The account assumes that, in dialogue, the linguistic representations employed by the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   261 citations  
  • Large Language Models: A Historical and Sociocultural Perspective.Eugene Yu Ji - 2024 - Cognitive Science 48 (3):e13430.
    This letter explores the intricate historical and contemporary links between large language models (LLMs) and cognitive science through the lens of information theory, statistical language models, and socioanthropological linguistic theories. The emergence of LLMs highlights the enduring significance of information‐based and statistical learning theories in understanding human communication. These theories, initially proposed in the mid‐20th century, offered a visionary framework for integrating computational science, social sciences, and humanities, which nonetheless was not fully fulfilled at that time. The subsequent development of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The pragmatic structure of refusal.Laura Caponetto - 2023 - Synthese 201 (6):1-19.
    This paper sets out to unpack the pragmatic structure of refusal—its illocutionary nature, success conditions, and normative effects. I argue that our ordinary concept of refusal captures a whole family of illocutions, comprising acts such as rejecting, declining, and the like, which share the property of being ‘negative second-turn illocutions’. Only _proper refusals_ (i.e. negative replies to permission requests), I submit, require speaker authority. I construe the ‘refusal family’ as a subclass of the directives-commissives intersection. After defending my view against (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Epistemic responsibility predicts developing frame awareness in early childhood: A language socialization perspective.Sarah Rose Bellavance - 2022 - Discourse Studies 24 (6):675-691.
    This article examines the emergent relationship between epistemic responsibility and frame awareness in early childhood, wherein a mother uses language socialization practices to guide her child into a new frame. The pair co-constructs the parameters of the new frame through negotiation of epistemic responsibility and remedial interchanges. The analysis demonstrates that these remedial interchanges arise from conflicting understandings of the embeddedness of frames and the epistemic dynamics that these frames entail. The child maintains epistemic primacy in her concurrent play frame, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Decision-making ethics in regards to life-sustaining interventions: when physicians refer to what other patients decide.Eve Rubli Truchard, Ralf J. Jox & Anca-Cristina Sterie - 2022 - BMC Medical Ethics 23 (1):1-13.
    BackgroundHealth decisions occur in a context with omnipresent social influences. Information concerning what other patients decide may present certain interventions as more desirable than others.ObjectivesTo explore how physicians refer to what other people decide in conversations about the relevancy of cardio-pulmonary resuscitation or do-not-attempt-resuscitation orders.MethodsWe recorded forty-three physician–patient admission interviews taking place in a hospital in French-speaking Switzerland, during which CPR is discussed. Data was analysed with conversation analysis.ResultsReference to what other people decide in regards to CPR is used five (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Face off – a semiotic technology study of software for making deepfakes.Søren Vigild Poulsen - 2021 - Sign Systems Studies 49 (3-4):489-508.
    Deepfakes, an algorithm that transposes the face of one person onto the face of another person in images and film, is a digital technology that may fundamentally alter our belief in visual modality and thus presents alarming consequences for an image-centric culture. Not only are these face-translations now so advanced that it is virtually impossible for people to tell that they are fake – this technology is also becoming accessible to laypersons who, with little or no computer skills, can use (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The tacit dimension of expertise: Professional vision at work in airport security.Chiara Bassetti - 2021 - Discourse Studies 23 (5):597-615.
    Whereas “professional vision” has been mostly analyzed in apprenticeship and other settings where knowledge is made explicit or reflected upon, I focus on how expertise tacitly plays out in task-oriented interaction among practitioners. The paper considers orientation both to the coworker’s and one’s own expertise in the collaborative accomplishment of airport security work. I show how screeners recruit action from colleagues in largely underspecified ways, based on shared access to the visibility field and expected professional vision. Requesting is tacitly accomplished (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Review article.[author unknown] - 1994 - Semiotica 99 (3-4):319-440.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • After post-cognitivism.Celia Kitzinger - 2006 - Discourse Studies 8 (1):67-83.
    This article briefly considers the convergence and divergence between Discursive Psychology and Conversation Analysis, in relation to cognition in talk-in-interaction. It explores the possibilities for research that begins from, rather than argues for, a post-cognitive perspective. Drawing in particular on an analysis of a single fragment of conversation, I suggest three analytic areas for researchers concerned both with talk-in-interaction and with cognition: i) the social organization of cognitive displays and embodiments; ii) the production of taken-for-granted culture through ‘internalized social norms’; (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Action formation and its epistemic (and other) backgrounds.John Heritage - 2012 - Discourse Studies 15 (5):551-578.
    This article reviews arguments that, in the process of action formation and ascription, the relative status of the participants with respect to a projected action can adjust or trump the action stance conveyed by the linguistic form of the utterance. In general, congruency between status and stance is preferred, and linguistic form is a fairly reliable guide to action ascription. However incongruities between stance and status result in action ascriptions that are at variance with the action stance that is otherwise (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • Categories in action: person-reference and membership categorization.Emanuel A. Schegloff - 2007 - Discourse Studies 9 (4):433-461.
    The article begins with an effort to clarify and differentiate a variety of terms used by analysts in dealing with mentions of persons in conversation and other forms of talk-in-interaction — such terms as person-reference, identifying, describing, categorizing, and the like. This effort leads to the observation that `reference to persons' and `membership categorization' are quite distinct sets of practices, with most reference to persons not being done by membership categories, and most uses of membership categorization devices being in the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Managing employees’ talk about problems in work in performance appraisal interviews.Jann Scheuer - 2014 - Discourse Studies 16 (3):407-429.
    Performance appraisal interviews are carried out on the basis of known-in-advance written materials such as preparation forms and interview guides. This article demonstrates how participants manage interviews by following a question–answer–response format fit to address interview guide entries one at a time. Two recurring supervisor responses to employees’ talk about problems in work are investigated: positive prediction and advice. It is suggested that these responses serve to establish supervisor authority and deter participants from discussing issues raised in employee answers and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the concept of action in the study of interaction.Jack Sidnell & N. J. Enfield - 2017 - Discourse Studies 19 (5):515-535.
    What is the relation between words and action? How does a person decide, based on what someone is saying, what would be an appropriate response? We argue that every move combines independent semiotic features, to be interpreted under an assumption that social behavior is goal directed; responding to actions is not equivalent to describing them; and describing actions invokes rights and duties for which people are explicitly accountable. We conclude that interaction does not involve a ‘binning’ procedure in which the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Can humans simulate talking like other humans? Comparing simulated clients to real customers in service inquiries.William Housley, Magnus Hamann, Saul Albert, Rein Ove Sikveland & Elizabeth Stokoe - 2020 - Discourse Studies 22 (1):87-109.
    How authentic are inquiry calls made by simulated clients, or ‘mystery shoppers’, to service organizations, when compared to real callers? We analysed 48 simulated and 63 real inquiry calls to different veterinary practices in the United Kingdom and Ireland. The data were transcribed for conversation analysis, as well as coded for a variety of call categories including reason for the call, call outcome and turn design features. Analysis revealed systematic differences between real and simulated calls in terms of reasons for (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mundane talk at work: Multiactivity in interactions between professionals and their clientele.Anne-Sylvie Horlacher & Elwys De Stefani - 2018 - Discourse Studies 20 (2):221-245.
    This article examines how participants coordinate concurrent activities in hair salon interactions and during driving lessons. In both settings, participants devote considerable time to chatting about mundane topics. This sort of conversation has traditionally been studied as an instance of small talk. The first part of the article retraces the epistemological origins of this notion. The analytical section shows how an analysis based on talk alone may lead researchers to distinguish small talk from task-directed talk, in line with previous studies. (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Actions in practice: On details in collections.Chase Wesley Raymond & Rebecca Clift - 2018 - Discourse Studies 20 (1):90-119.
    Several of the contributions to the Lynch et al. Special issue make the claim that conversation-analytic research into epistemics is ‘routinely crafted at the expense of actual, produced and constitutive detail, and what that detail may show us’. Here, we seek to address the inappositeness of this critique by tracing precisely how it is that recognizable actions emerge from distinct practices of interaction. We begin by reviewing some of the foundational tenets of conversation-analytic theory and method – including the relationship (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Reference as an Interactive Achievement: Sequential and Longitudinal Analyses of Labeling Interactions in Shared Book Reading and Free Play.Vivien Heller & Katharina J. Rohlfing - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Experimental Philosophy, Ethnomethodology, and Intentional Action: A Textual Analysis of the Knobe Effect.Gustav Lymer & Olle Blomberg - 2019 - Human Studies 42 (4):673-694.
    In “Intentional action and side-effects in ordinary language” (2003), Joshua Knobe reported an asymmetry in test subjects’ responses to a question about intentionality: subjects are more likely to judge that a side effect of an agent’s intended action is intentional if they think the side effect is morally bad than if they think it is morally good. This result has been taken to suggest that the concept of intentionality is an inherently moral concept. In this paper, we draw attention to (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation