Switch to: References

Citations of:

On Growth and Form [Book Review]

Journal of Philosophy 42 (20):557-558 (1945)

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Comparison between the work of synthetic biologists and the action of evolution: engineering versus tinkering.Michel Morange - 2013 - Biological Theory 8 (4):318-323.
    The comparison between natural evolution and the action of a tinkerer has become highly popular since its reintroduction by François Jacob at the end of the 1970s. It has been used as a weapon against the existence of an “intelligent design” as well as a way for synthetic biologists to promote their ambitious projects. I will describe the complex history of this metaphor, and examine its pertinence. Whereas Darwin considered it as a way to describe how evolution proceeded, Jacob linked (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Hemisphere differences before 1800.Gert-Jan C. Lokhorst - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):642-642.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Finding the way in phenotypic space: the origin and maintenance of constraints on organismal form.Massimo Pigliucci - 2007 - Annals of Botany 100:433-438.
    Background: One of the all-time questions in evolutionary biology regards the evolution of organismal shapes, and in particular why certain forms appear repeatedly in the history of life, others only seldom and still others not at all. Recent research in this field has deployed the conceptual framework of constraints and natural selection as measured by quantitative genetic methods. Scope: In this paper I argue that quantitative genetics can by necessity only provide us with useful statistical sum- maries that may lead (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • What's not wrong with libertarianism: Reply to Friedman.Tom G. Palmer - 1998 - Critical Review: A Journal of Politics and Society 12 (3):337-358.
    Abstract In his critique of modern libertarian thinking, Jeffrey Friedman (1997) argues that libertarian moral theory makes social science irrelevant. However, if its moral claims are hypothetical rather than categorical imperatives, then economics, history, sociology, and other disciplines play a central role in libertarian thought. Limitations on human knowledge necessitate abstractly formulated rules, among which are claims of rights. Further, Friedman's remarks on freedom rest on an erroneous understanding of the role of definitions in philosophy, and his characterization of the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A patterned process approach to brain, consciousness, and behavior.José-Luis Díaz - 1997 - Philosophical Psychology 10 (2):179-195.
    The architecture of brain, consciousness, and behavioral processes is shown to be formally similar in that all three may be conceived and depicted as Petri net patterned processes structured by a series of elements occurring or becoming active in stochastic succession, in parallel, with different rhythms of temporal iteration, and with a distinct qualitative manifestation in the spatiotemporal domain. A patterned process theory is derived from the isomorphic features of the models and contrasted with connectionist, dynamic system notions. This empirically (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The redoubtable cell.Andrew Reynolds - 2010 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 41 (3):194-201.
    The cell theory—the thesis that all life is made up of one or more cells, the fundamental structural and physiological unit—is one of the most celebrated achievements of modern biological science. And yet from its very inception in the nineteenth century it has faced repeated criticism from some biologists. Why do some continue to criticize the cell theory, and how has it managed nevertheless to keep burying its undertakers? The answers to these questions reveal the complex nature of the cell (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Explaining the properties of organisms.Vernon Pratt - 1974 - Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 5 (1):1-15.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Return of the hopeful monster.Stephen Jay Gould - unknown
    ig Brother, the tyrant of George Orwell's 1984, directed his daily Two Minutes Hate against Emmanuel Goldstein, enemy of the people. When I studied evolutionary biology in graduate school during the mid 1960s, official rebuke and derision focused upon Richard Goldschmidt , a famous geneticist who, we were told, had gone astray. Although 1984 creeps up on us, I trust that the world will not be in Big Brother's grip by then. I do, however, predict that during this decade Goldschmidt (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • A model proposed for the process of evolution with special reference to plants.Harald Riedl - 1972 - Acta Biotheoretica 21 (1-2):63-85.
    A spiral running along the surface of a cone standing on top is proposed as a model for evolution, the progress of which is considered as composed of a linear progression following the direction of time and representing the linear increase in the number of taxa, while a circular component stands for the ever recurring functional types of organisms.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Allometry and geometry ofbegonia leaves.Bernard Jeune & Denis Barabé - 1995 - Acta Biotheoretica 43 (3):205-215.
    The authors constructed an algorithm that relates leaf contour to the ratio between the two parts of the leaf using the function θ’=nθm, wherem is the allometric exponent. Using this model, it is possible to simulate the contour of symmetrical or asymmetrical leaves. The authors hypothesize that the portion of the leaf contour that agrees with the simulation is linked to a constraint imposed by the initial asymmetry of the leaf primordium. The final shape of the leaf results more from (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Behavior, body types and the irreversibility of evolution.Francisco Aboitiz - 1990 - Acta Biotheoretica 38 (2):91-101.
    A functional approach to evolutionary morphology is emphasized in this paper. This perspective differs from the current structuralist trend, which emphasizes the constraining role of developmental paths. In addition, the present approach agrees with the adaptationist paradigm. It is further argued that three types of phenomena are better understood in this light: i.- the existence of evolutionary trends, ii.- the maintenance of certain structural features within a given taxon, and iii.- the irreversibility of evolution.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Nineteenth-century ideas on hemisphere differences and "duality of mind".Anne Harrington - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):617-660.
    It is widely felt that the sorts of ideas current in modern laterality and split-brain research are largely without precedent in the behavioral and brain sciences. This paper not only challenges that view, but makes a first attempt to define the relevance of older concepts and data to present research programs.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • ‘The Action of the Brain’. Machine Models and Adaptive Functions in Turing and Ashby.Hajo Greif - 2017 - In Vincent C. Müller (ed.), Philosophy and theory of artificial intelligence 2017. Berlin: Springer. pp. 24-35.
    Given the personal acquaintance between Alan M. Turing and W. Ross Ashby and the partial proximity of their research fields, a comparative view of Turing’s and Ashby’s work on modelling “the action of the brain” (letter from Turing to Ashby, 1946) will help to shed light on the seemingly strict symbolic/embodied dichotomy: While it is clear that Turing was committed to formal, computational and Ashby to material, analogue methods of modelling, there is no straightforward mapping of these approaches onto symbol-based (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Chreods, homeorhesis and biofields: Finding the right path for science.Arran Gare - 2017 - Progress in Biophysics and Molecular Biology 131:61-91.
    C.H. Waddington’s concepts of ‘chreods’ (canalized paths of development) and ‘homeorhesis’ (the tendency to return to a path), each associated with ‘morphogenetic fields’, were conceived by him as a contribution to complexity theory. Subsequent developments in complexity theory have largely ignored Waddington’s work and efforts to advance it. Waddington explained the development of the concept of chreod as the influence on his work of Alfred North Whitehead’s process philosophy, notably, the concept of concrescence as a self-causing process. Processes were recognized (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Reconsidering Morphology Through an Experimental Case Study.Liliana Albertazzi, Luisa Canal, Paolo Chistè, Mara De Rosa, Rocco Micciolo & Alessandro Minelli - 2017 - Biological Theory 12 (3):131-141.
    This study analyzes shells of marine gastropods of a zoological museum and the Latin epithets expressing perceptual and connotative attributes that they have received in the standard, Linnaean nomenclature. Making use of the Osgood semantic differential, we presented the subjects with digital 3-D reproductions of the shell specimens to be subjectively evaluated according to 17 pairs of attributes. The results show that, overall, the subjective evaluations given by the subjects are consistent, which suggests that an intersubjective characterization of the shells (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • A Morphology of Theories of Emergence.Ritchey Tom - 2014 - In Tom Ritchey (ed.), Acta Morphologica Generalis. Acta Morphologica Generalis.
    “Emergence” – the notion of novel, unpredictable and irreducible properties developing out of complex organisational entities – is itself a complex, multi-dimensional concept. To date there is no single, generally agreed upon “theory of emergence”, but instead a number of different approaches and perspectives. Neither is there a common conceptual or meta-theoretical framework by which to systematically identify, exemplify and compare different “theories”. Building upon earlier work done by sociologist Kenneth Bailey, this article presents a method for creating such a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Language as an instrument of thought.Eran Asoulin - 2016 - Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 1 (1):1-23.
    I show that there are good arguments and evidence to boot that support the language as an instrument of thought hypothesis. The underlying mechanisms of language, comprising of expressions structured hierarchically and recursively, provide a perspective (in the form of a conceptual structure) on the world, for it is only via language that certain perspectives are avail- able to us and to our thought processes. These mechanisms provide us with a uniquely human way of thinking and talking about the world (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Constraint‐Based Reasoning for Search and Explanation: Strategies for Understanding Variation and Patterns in Biology.Sara Green & Nicholaos Jones - 2016 - Dialectica 70 (3):343-374.
    Life scientists increasingly rely upon abstraction-based modeling and reasoning strategies for understanding biological phenomena. We introduce the notion of constraint-based reasoning as a fruitful tool for conceptualizing some of these developments. One important role of mathematical abstractions is to impose formal constraints on a search space for possible hypotheses and thereby guide the search for plausible causal models. Formal constraints are, however, not only tools for biological explanations but can be explanatory by virtue of clarifying general dependency-relations and patterning between (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • The Philosophy of Biomimicry.Henry Dicks - 2016 - Philosophy and Technology 29 (3):223-243.
    The philosophy of biomimicry, I argue, consists of four main areas of inquiry. The first, which has already been explored by Freya Mathews, concerns the “deep” question of what Nature ultimately is. The second, third, and fourth areas correspond to the three basic principles of biomimicry as laid out by Janine Benyus. “Nature as model” is the poetic principle of biomimicry, for it tells us how it is that things are to be “brought forth”. “Nature as measure” is the ethical (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  • The need for synthetic cognitive development theory.William M. Bart - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):137-138.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A mobility gradient in the organization of vertebrate movement: The perception of movement through symbolic language.Ilan Golani - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):249-266.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • The natural geometry of a behavioral homology.Ilan Golani - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):291-308.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Shapes of behaviour.John G. Harries - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):279-281.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • From psychopharmacology to neuropsychopharmacology: Adapting behavioral terminology to neural events.George V. Rebec - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):287-288.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Brain theory and the uses of history.Samuel H. Greenblatt - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):637-638.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The role of cerebral lateralization in expression of spatial cognition.Halle D. Brown - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):240-241.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Darwin´s two hundred years: is not time for a change?Armando Aranda-Anzaldo - 2009 - Ludus Vitalis 17 (32):87-99.
    Two hundred years after Darwin’s birth, the evolution of living systems is an accepted fact but there is scope for controversy on the mechanisms involved in such a process. Mainstream neo-Darwinism champions the role of natural selection (NS) as the fundamental cause of the evolutionary process as well as of random, contingent events at the genetic level as the main source of variation upon which NS performs its causal role. Thus, according to neo-Darwinism the course of biological evolution is quite (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The exaptive excellence of spandrels as a term and prototype.Stephen Jay Gould - unknown
    In 1979, Lewontin and I borrowed the archi- tectural term “spandrel” (using the pendentives of San Marco in Venice as an example) to designate the class of forms and spaces that arise as necessary byproducts of another decision in design, and not as adaptations for direct utility in them- selves. This proposal has generated a large literature featur- ing two critiques: (i) the terminological claim that the span- drels of San Marco are not true spandrels at all and (ii) the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   29 citations  
  • Biology Clearly Needs Morphometrics. Does Morphometrics Need Biology?Charles Oxnard & Paul O’Higgins - 2009 - Biological Theory 4 (1):84-97.
    It is now well documented that biology needs morphometrics. Morphometrics can provide useful and often unexpected information about development and growth, functional—especially mechanical—adaptation, and evolutionary difference and relationship. Such studies often apply coordinate data from anatomical landmarks. Recently semi-landmarks and sliding landmarks increase information content, especially of apparently featureless regions . Yet, how we landmark our materials limits the results we get and the questions we ask. Here we show different landmarking schemes leading to different equivalences between specimens and different (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Rational taxonomy and the natural system.Mae-Wan Ho & Peter T. Saunders - 1993 - Acta Biotheoretica 41 (4):289-304.
    Since Darwin's theory of evolution by natural selection, the idea of descent with modification came to dominate systematics, and so the study of morphology became subgugated to the reconstruction of phylogenies. Reinstating the organism in the theory of evolution (Ho & Saunders, 1979; Webster & Goodwin, 1982) leads to a project inrational taxonomy (Ho, 1986, 1988a), which attempts to classify biological forms on the basis of transformations on a given dynamical structure.Does rational taxonomy correspond to thenatural system that Linnaeus and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Mills made of grist, and other interesting ideas in need of clarification.Paul E. Smaldino & Michael J. Spivey - 2019 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 42:e182.
    Heyes’ book is an important contribution that rightly integrates cognitive development and cultural evolution. However, understanding the cultural evolution of cognitive gadgets requires a deeper appreciation of complexity, feedback, and self-organization than her book exhibits.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Superordinate shape classification using natural shape statistics.John Wilder, Jacob Feldman & Manish Singh - 2011 - Cognition 119 (3):325-340.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • A history of character concepts in evolutionary biology.Kurt M. Fristrup - 2000 - In Günter P. Wagner (ed.), The Character Concept in Evolutionary Biology. Academic Press. pp. 15--37.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Elastic growth in thin geometries.J. Dervaux & M. Ben Amar - 2009 - In Maryvonne Gérin & Marie-Christine Maurel (eds.), Origins of Life: Self-Organization and/or Biological Evolution? EDP Sciences. pp. 79--94.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Scaling, uniqueness, and integration.John W. Gyr - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):141-142.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The environment modulates the mobility gradient, temporally if not sequentially.Charles H. M. Beck - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):268-269.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Sensorimotor reference frames and physiological attractors.René Thom - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):289-289.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Structure and function in the CNS.Peter H. Klopfer - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):281-282.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The origin of cellular life.Donald E. Ingber - 2000 - Bioessays 22 (12):1160-1170.
    This essay presents a scenario of the origin of life that is based on analysis of biological architecture and mechanical design at the microstructural level. My thesis is that the same architectural and energetic constraints that shape cells today also guided the evolution of the first cells and that the molecular scaffolds that support solid-phase biochemistry in modern cells represent living microfossils of past life forms. This concept emerged from the discovery that cells mechanically stabilize themselves using tensegrity architecture and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The many-mind problem: Neuroscience or neurotheology?John C. Marshall - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):642-643.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Models in Biology and Physics: What’s the Difference?Darrell Patrick Rowbottom - 2009 - Foundations of Science 14 (4):281-294.
    In Making Sense of Life , Keller emphasizes several differences between biology and physics. Her analysis focuses on significant ways in which modelling practices in some areas of biology, especially developmental biology, differ from those of the physical sciences. She suggests that natural models and modelling by homology play a central role in the former but not the latter. In this paper, I focus instead on those practices that are importantly similar, from the point of view of epistemology and cognitive (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Brain readiness and the nature of language.Denis Bouchard - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6:158611.
    To identify the neural components that make a brain ready for language, it is important to have well defined linguistic phenotypes, to know precisely what language is. There are two central features to language: the capacity to form signs (words), and the capacity to combine them into complex structures. We must determine how the human brain enables these capacities. A sign is a link between a perceptual form and a conceptual meaning. Acoustic elements and content elements, are already brain-internal in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Environmental factors and the organization of developmental changes.Barbara Koslowski - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):143-144.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Striatal structures, dopamine and the mobility gradient model.Alexander R. Cools - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):271-272.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Animal motility: Gestalt or piecemeal assembly.Paul Leyhausen - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):282-282.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Dynamical systems theory and the mobility gradient: Information, homology and self-similar structure.Gary Goldberg - 1992 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15 (2):278-279.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Generative versus nongenerative thought.Michael C. Corballis - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):242-243.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • There is more to location than prepositions.David C. Bennett - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):239-239.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Micelle formation and crystallization as paradigms for virus assembly.Alexander McPherson - 2005 - Bioessays 27 (4):447-458.
    Self-assembly processes of crystallization, micelle formation and virus assembly, by their creation of geometric order from disordered components, represent first-order phase transitions that arise through the formation of partially ordered intermediates. The self-assembly of protein subunits into the geometric shells of polyhedral viruses may proceed through formation of reverse micelles, and be driven by condensation of encapsidated nucleic acid complexed with the amino terminal polypeptides of the coat proteins. Restructuring of subunits on the fluid, micellar surface, analogous to processes on (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Semiokinesis Semiotic autopoiesis of the Universe.Abir U. Igamberdiev - 2001 - Semiotica 2001 (135):1-23.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations