Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Representation is representation of similarities.Shimon Edelman - 1998 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (4):449-467.
    Intelligent systems are faced with the problem of securing a principled (ideally, veridical) relationship between the world and its internal representation. I propose a unified approach to visual representation, addressing both the needs of superordinate and basic-level categorization and of identification of specific instances of familiar categories. According to the proposed theory, a shape is represented by its similarity to a number of reference shapes, measured in a high-dimensional space of elementary features. This amounts to embedding the stimulus in a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations  
  • Afordancje dla robotów: krótki przegląd.Robert St Amant, Arpan Chakraborty & Thomas E. Horton - 2014 - Avant: Trends in Interdisciplinary Studies 5 (1):133-150.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • When do representations explain?Daniel C. Dennett - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):406.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • On the hypothesis that grammars are mentally represented.William Demopoulos & Robert J. Matthews - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):405-406.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   36 citations  
  • On some fundamental distinctions of computationalism.William Demopoulos - 1987 - Synthese 70 (January):79-96.
    The following paper presents a characterization of three distinctions fundamental to computationalism, viz., the distinction between analog and digital machines, representation and nonrepresentation-using systems, and direct and indirect perceptual processes. Each distinction is shown to rest on nothing more than the methodological principles which justify the explanatory framework of the special sciences.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The how and why of what went where in apparent motion: Modeling solutions to the motion correspondence problem.Michael R. Dawson - 1991 - Psychological Review 98 (4):569-603.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   30 citations  
  • Direct perception theory needs to include computational reasoning, not extraretinal information.Niels da Vitoria Lobo - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):318-318.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Church's thesis and representation of grammars.Martin Davis - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):404-404.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A history of subliminal perception in autobiography.Robert G. Crowder - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):28-29.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Temporal representation in the control of movement.Daniel M. Corcos - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):206-206.
    Theories of the representation of specific kinetic and spatiotem-poral features of movement range from the explicit assertion that temporal aspects of movement are not represented to the idea that they are represented and that they have neurophysiological correlates. Jeannerod's thesis is that mental and visual images have common mechanisms and that there is a link between the image to move and the mechanisms involved with movement. The target article takes the position that certain parameters are coded in motor representations but (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Electrodermal responses to words in an irrelevant message: A partial reappraisal.Raymond S. Corteen - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):27-28.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Sensor fusion in motion perception.David Coombs - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):317-318.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The creative brain: Symmetry breaking in motor imagery.José L. Contreras-Vidal, Jean P. Banquet, Jany Brebion & Mark J. Smith - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):204-205.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Cognitive and motor implications of mental imagery.Romeo Chua & Daniel J. Weeks - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):203-204.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • The paradox of social interaction: Shared intentionality, we-reasoning, and virtual bargaining.Nick Chater, Hossam Zeitoun & Tigran Melkonyan - 2022 - Psychological Review 129 (3):415-437.
    Social interaction is both ubiquitous and central to understanding human behavior. Such interactions depend, we argue, on shared intentionality: the parties must form a common understanding of an ambiguous interaction. Yet how can shared intentionality arise? Many well-known accounts of social cognition, including those involving “mind-reading,” typically fall into circularity and/or regress. For example, A’s beliefs and behavior may depend on her prediction of B’s beliefs and behavior, but B’s beliefs and behavior depend in turn on her prediction of A’s (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Now you see it, now you don't: Relations between semantic activation and awareness.Thomas H. Carr & Dale Dagenbach - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):26-27.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Bergson and Athleticism.Geoffrey Callaghan - 2013 - Sport, Ethics and Philosophy 7 (2):231-244.
    The work of Henri Bergson has gone almost completely unnoticed in philosophy of sport literature. This in no way indicates the level of relevance his programme may carry for the subject. Many of the entrenched debates that have historically helped to shape the field are mirrored by Bergson's own concerns regarding perception and skill acquisition. As such, a thorough study of how the Bergsonian programme might approach the topic of athletic action is in no wise an idle pursuit? in fact, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Representation and computation in a deflationary assessment of connectionist cognitive science.Keith Butler - 1995 - Synthese 104 (1):71-97.
    Connectionism provides hope for unifying work in neuroscience, computer science, and cognitive psychology. This promise has met with some resistance from Classical Computionalists, which may have inspired Connectionists to retaliate with bold, inflationary claims on behalf of Connectionist models. This paper demonstrates, by examining three intimately connected issues, that these inflationary claims made on behalf of Connectionism are wrong. This should not be construed as an attack on Connectionism, however, since the inflated claims made on its behalf have the look (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On the limitations of imaging imagining.Christopher A. Buneo & Martha Flanders - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):202-203.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Ego- and object-motion perception: Where does it take place?U. Büttner & A. Straube - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):316-317.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Theories of visual masking.Bruce Bridgeman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):25-26.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Extending reference signal theory to rapid movements.Bruce Bridgeman & Jean Blouin - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):315-316.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • On a computational perspective without substance.Rudolf P. Botha - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):403-404.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Biological perception of self-motion.Ronald G. Boothe - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):314-315.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unarticulated meaning.James Blachowicz - 1994 - Erkenntnis 40 (1):43 - 70.
    It is a common experience of mental life that we come to articulate meanings which we had initially grasped in only a sketchy way. In this paper, I consider how this idea of an initially unarticulated meaning may fit in a general theory of mental representation. I propose to identify unarticulated meanings with what I callspecific concepts, which are quite similar to Rosch's categories of basic objects and are distinct both from images and generic concepts (which come to articulate meanings). (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Through the looking-glass and what cognitive psychology found there.Edoardo Bisiach - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):24-25.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Using what you know: A computer-science perspective.Robert C. Berwick - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):402-403.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Frame and metrics for the reference signal.Victor I. Belopolsky - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):313-314.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A theory of the perceptual stability of the visual world rather than of motion perception.Wolfgang Becker & Thomas Mergner - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):312-313.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Unconscious semantic processing: The pendulum keeps on swinging.David A. Balota - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):23-24.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Do object affordances represent the functionality of an object?Ruzena Bajcsy - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):202-202.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Analysis of information for 3-D motion perception: The role of eye movements.George J. Andersen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):311-312.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Two psychologies of perception and the prospect of their synthesis.Sergei Gepshtein - 2010 - Philosophical Psychology 23 (2):217 – 281.
    Two traditions have had a great impact on the theoretical and experimental research of perception. One tradition is statistical, stretching from Fechner's enunciation of psychophysics in 1860 to the modern view of perception as statistical decision making. The other tradition is phenomenological, from Brentano's “empirical standpoint” of 1874 to the Gestalt movement and the modern work on perceptual organization. Each tradition has at its core a distinctive assumption about the indivisible constituents of perception: the just-noticeable differences of sensation in the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Extending the notion of affordance.Silvano Zipoli Caiani - 2014 - Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 13 (2):275-293.
    Post-Gibson attempts to set out a definition of affordance generally agree that this notion can be understood as a property of the environment with salience for an organism’s behavior. According to this view, some scholars advocate the idea that affordances are dispositional properties of physical objects that, given suitable circumstances, necessarily actualize related actions. This paper aims at assessing this statement in light of a theory of affordance perception. After years of discontinuity between strands of empirical and theoretical research, the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Practices and the Direct Perception of Normative States.Julie Zahle - 2013 - Philosophy of the Social Sciences 43 (4):493-518.
    The overall aim of this two-part article is to provide a supplement to ability theories of practice in terms of a defense of the following thesis: In situations of social interaction, individuals’ ability to act appropriately sometimes depends on their exercise of the ability directly to perceive normative states. In this Part I, I introduce ability theories of practice and motivate my thesis. Furthermore, I offer an analysis of normative states as response-dependent properties. Last, I work out and defend an (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The significance of the active pick-up of information in ecological theories of motion perception.Lucy Yardley - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):340-340.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • To what extent do beliefs affect apparent motion?Richard D. Wright & Michael R. W. Dawson - 1994 - Philosophical Psychology 7 (4):471-491.
    A number of studies in the apparent motion literature were examined using the cognitive penetrability criterion to determine the extent to which beliefs affect the perception of apparent motion. It was found that the interaction between the perceptual processes mediating apparent motion and higher order processes appears to be limited. In addition, perceptual and inferential beliefs appear to have different effects on perceived motion optimality and direction. Our findings suggest that the system underlying apparent motion perception has more than one (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • A review of the literature with and without awareness. [REVIEW]George Wolford - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):49-50.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Rules are not processes.Robert Wilensky - 1983 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (3):415.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Prospects for direct social perception: a multi-theoretical integration to further the science of social cognition.Travis J. Wiltshire, Emilio J. C. Lobato, Daniel S. McConnell & Stephen M. Fiore - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8:100549.
    In this paper we suggest that differing approaches to the science of social cognition mirror the arguments between radical embodied and traditional approaches to cognition. We contrast the use in social cognition of theoretical inference and mental simulation mechanisms with approaches emphasizing a direct perception of others’ mental states. We build from a recent integrative framework unifying these divergent perspectives through the use of dual-process theory and supporting social neuroscience research. Our elaboration considers two complementary notions of direct perception: one (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • The mystery-mastery-imagery complex.H. T. A. Whiting & R. P. Ingvaldsen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):228-229.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Motion perception: Rights, wrongs and further speculations.Alexander H. Wertheim - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):340-355.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Motion perception during selfmotion: The direct versus inferential controversy revisited.Alexander H. Wertheim - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):293-311.
    According to the traditional inferential theory of perception, percepts of object motion or stationarity stem from an evaluation of afferent retinal signals (which encode image motion) with the help of extraretinal signals (which encode eye movements). According to direct perception theory, on the other hand, the percepts derive from retinally conveyed information only. Neither view is compatible with a perceptual phenomenon that occurs during visually induced sensations of ego motion (vection). A modified version of inferential theory yields a model in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The illusion of self-motion in virtual reality environments.John Wann & Simon Rushton - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):338-340.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Potential disparities between imagining and preparing motor skills.Charles B. Walter & Stephan P. Swinnen - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):227-228.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Imagery needs preparation too.Stefan Vogt - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):226-227.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Space as reference signal? Elaborate it in depth!Boris M. Velichkovsky & A. H. C. Van der Heijden - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):337-338.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Perceivable information or: The happy marriage between ecological psychology and gestalt.Cees van Leeuwen & John Stins - 1994 - Philosophical Psychology 7 (2):267-285.
    The ecological realist concept of information as environmental specification is discussed. It is argued that affordances in ecological realism could, in principle, rest on a notion of partial specification of environmental circumstances. For this aim, a notion of Gestalt quality as a hierarchical structure of affordances would have to be adopted. It is claimed that such an account could provide a promising way to deal with problems of intentionality in perception and action, awareness and problem solving.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Action and attention.A. H. C. Van der Heijden & Bruce Bridgeman - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (2):225-226.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Facilitation or inhibition from parafoveal words?Geoffrey Underwood - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):48-49.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark