Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small.Jeffrey L. Elman - 1993 - Cognition 48 (1):71-99.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   239 citations  
  • Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems.David R. Shanks & Mark F. St John - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):367-447.
    A number of ways of taxonomizing human learning have been proposed. We examine the evidence for one such proposal, namely, that there exist independent explicit and implicit learning systems. This combines two further distinctions, (1) between learning that takes place with versus without concurrent awareness, and (2) between learning that involves the encoding of instances (or fragments) versus the induction of abstract rules or hypotheses. Implicit learning is assumed to involve unconscious rule learning. We examine the evidence for implicit learning (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   216 citations  
  • Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans.R. I. M. Dunbar - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):681-694.
    Group size is a function of relative neocortical volume in nonhuman primates. Extrapolation from this regression equation yields a predicted group size for modern humans very similar to that of certain hunter-gatherer and traditional horticulturalist societies. Groups of similar size are also found in other large-scale forms of contemporary and historical society. Among primates, the cohesion of groups is maintained by social grooming; the time devoted to social grooming is linearly related to group size among the Old World monkeys and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   243 citations  
  • (1 other version)Homo Heuristicus: Why Biased Minds Make Better Inferences.Gerd Gigerenzer & Henry Brighton - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (1):107-143.
    Heuristics are efficient cognitive processes that ignore information. In contrast to the widely held view that less processing reduces accuracy, the study of heuristics shows that less information, computation, and time can in fact improve accuracy. We review the major progress made so far: the discovery of less-is-more effects; the study of the ecological rationality of heuristics, which examines in which environments a given strategy succeeds or fails, and why; an advancement from vague labels to computational models of heuristics; the (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   164 citations  
  • Bayesian Fundamentalism or Enlightenment? On the explanatory status and theoretical contributions of Bayesian models of cognition.Matt Jones & Bradley C. Love - 2011 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34 (4):169-188.
    The prominence of Bayesian modeling of cognition has increased recently largely because of mathematical advances in specifying and deriving predictions from complex probabilistic models. Much of this research aims to demonstrate that cognitive behavior can be explained from rational principles alone, without recourse to psychological or neurological processes and representations. We note commonalities between this rational approach and other movements in psychology – namely, Behaviorism and evolutionary psychology – that set aside mechanistic explanations or make use of optimality assumptions. Through (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   130 citations  
  • Semantics without semantic content.Daniel W. Harris - 2020 - Mind and Language 37 (3):304-328.
    I argue that semantics is the study of the proprietary database of a centrally inaccessible and informationally encapsulated input–output system. This system’s role is to encode and decode partial and defeasible evidence of what speakers are saying. Since information about nonlinguistic context is therefore outside the purview of semantic processing, a sentence’s semantic value is not its content but a partial and defeasible constraint on what it can be used to say. I show how to translate this thesis into a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Language acquisition in the absence of experience.Stephen Crain - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):597-612.
    A fundamental goal of linguistic theory is to explain how natural languages are acquired. This paper describes some recent findings on how learners acquire syntactic knowledge for which there is little, if any, decisive evidence from the environment. The first section presents several general observations about language acquisition that linguistic theory has tried to explain and discusses the thesis that certain linguistic properties are innate because they appear universally and in the absence of corresponding experience. A third diagnostic for innateness, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   115 citations  
  • On the evolution of language and generativity.Michael C. Corballis - 1992 - Cognition 44 (3):197-226.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   116 citations  
  • The neural basis of cognitive development: A constructivist manifesto.Steven R. Quartz & Terrence J. Sejnowski - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (4):537-556.
    How do minds emerge from developing brains? According to the representational features of cortex are built from the dynamic interaction between neural growth mechanisms and environmentally derived neural activity. Contrary to popular selectionist models that emphasize regressive mechanisms, the neurobiological evidence suggests that this growth is a progressive increase in the representational properties of cortex. The interaction between the environment and neural growth results in a flexible type of learning: minimizes the need for prespecification in accordance with recent neurobiological evidence (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   108 citations  
  • Human simulations of vocabulary learning.Jane Gillette, Henry Gleitman, Lila Gleitman & Anne Lederer - 1999 - Cognition 73 (2):135-176.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   84 citations  
  • Coordinating perceptually grounded categories through language: A case study for colour.Luc Steels & Tony Belpaeme - 2005 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28 (4):469-489.
    This article proposes a number of models to examine through which mechanisms a population of autonomous agents could arrive at a repertoire of perceptually grounded categories that is sufficiently shared to allow successful communication. The models are inspired by the main approaches to human categorisation being discussed in the literature: nativism, empiricism, and culturalism. Colour is taken as a case study. Although we take no stance on which position is to be accepted as final truth with respect to human categorisation (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   58 citations  
  • A critical period for second language acquisition: Evidence from 2/3 million English speakers.Joshua K. Hartshorne, Joshua B. Tenenbaum & Steven Pinker - 2018 - Cognition 177 (C):263-277.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  • Modeling human performance in statistical word segmentation.Michael C. Frank, Sharon Goldwater, Thomas L. Griffiths & Joshua B. Tenenbaum - 2010 - Cognition 117 (2):107-125.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Trading spaces: Computation, representation, and the limits of uninformed learning.Andy Clark & Chris Thornton - 1997 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20 (1):57-66.
    Some regularities enjoy only an attenuated existence in a body of training data. These are regularities whose statistical visibility depends on some systematic recoding of the data. The space of possible recodings is, however, infinitely large – it is the space of applicable Turing machines. As a result, mappings that pivot on such attenuated regularities cannot, in general, be found by brute-force search. The class of problems that present such mappings we call the class of “type-2 problems.” Type-1 problems, by (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • Neural networks, nativism, and the plausibility of constructivism.Steven R. Quartz - 1993 - Cognition 48 (3):223-242.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Granularity and the acquisition of grammatical gender: How order-of-acquisition affects what gets learned.Inbal Arnon & Michael Ramscar - 2012 - Cognition 122 (3):292-305.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • The cognitive roots of regularization in language.Vanessa Ferdinand, Simon Kirby & Kenny Smith - 2019 - Cognition 184 (C):53-68.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Can resources save rationality? ‘Anti-Bayesian’ updating in cognition and perception.Eric Mandelbaum, Isabel Won, Steven Gross & Chaz Firestone - 2020 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 143:e16.
    Resource rationality may explain suboptimal patterns of reasoning; but what of “anti-Bayesian” effects where the mind updates in a direction opposite the one it should? We present two phenomena — belief polarization and the size-weight illusion — that are not obviously explained by performance- or resource-based constraints, nor by the authors’ brief discussion of reference repulsion. Can resource rationality accommodate them?
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • (1 other version)Balancing Effort and Information Transmission During Language Acquisition: Evidence From Word Order and Case Marking.Maryia Fedzechkina, Elissa L. Newport & T. Florian Jaeger - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (2):416-446.
    Across languages of the world, some grammatical patterns have been argued to be more common than expected by chance. These are sometimes referred to as (statistical) language universals. One such universal is the correlation between constituent order freedom and the presence of a case system in a language. Here, we explore whether this correlation can be explained by a bias to balance production effort and informativity of cues to grammatical function. Two groups of learners were presented with miniature artificial languages (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • The distributional structure of grammatical categories in speech to young children.Toben H. Mintz, Elissa L. Newport & Thomas G. Bever - 2002 - Cognitive Science 26 (4):393-424.
    We present a series of three analyses of young children's linguistic input to determine the distributional information it could plausibly offer to the process of grammatical category learning. Each analysis was conducted on four separate corpora from the CHILDES database (MacWhinney, 2000) of speech directed to children under 2;5. We showthat, in accord with other findings, a distributional analysis which categorizeswords based on their co‐occurrence patterns with surroundingwords successfully categorizes the majority of nouns and verbs. In Analyses 2 and 3, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   34 citations  
  • (1 other version)Balancing Effort and Information Transmission During Language Acquisition: Evidence From Word Order and Case Marking.Maryia Fedzechkina, Elissa L. Newport & T. Florian Jaeger - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (6):n/a-n/a.
    Across languages of the world, some grammatical patterns have been argued to be more common than expected by chance. These are sometimes referred to as language universals. One such universal is the correlation between constituent order freedom and the presence of a case system in a language. Here, we explore whether this correlation can be explained by a bias to balance production effort and informativity of cues to grammatical function. Two groups of learners were presented with miniature artificial languages containing (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: how important is starting small?Douglas L. T. Rohde & David C. Plaut - 1999 - Cognition 72 (1):67-109.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   32 citations  
  • The role of abstract syntactic knowledge in language acquisition: a reply to Tomasello.Cynthia Fisher - 2002 - Cognition 82 (3):259-278.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   24 citations  
  • When we think about thinking: The acquisition of belief verbs.Anna Papafragou - 2007 - Cognition 105 (1):125.
    Mental-content verbs such as think, believe, imagine and hope seem to pose special problems for the young language learner. One possible explanation for these diYculties is that the concepts that these verbs express are hard to grasp and therefore their acquisition must await relevant conceptual development. According to a diVerent, perhaps complementary, proposal, a major contributor to the diYculty of these items lies with the informational requirements for identifying them from the contexts in which they appear. The experiments reported here (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   26 citations  
  • Cognitive influences in language evolution: Psycholinguistic predictors of loan word borrowing.Padraic Monaghan & Seán G. Roberts - 2019 - Cognition 186 (C):147-158.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • What exactly is Universal Grammar, and has anyone seen it?Ewa Dąbrowska - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Neurocognitive mechanisms of statistical-sequential learning: what do event-related potentials tell us?Jerome Daltrozzo & Christopher M. Conway - 2014 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Through a narrow window: working memory capacity and the detection of covariation.Yaakov Kareev - 1995 - Cognition 56 (3):263-269.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   25 citations  
  • The impact of adjacent-dependencies and staged-input on the learnability of center-embedded hierarchical structures.Jun Lai & Fenna H. Poletiek - 2011 - Cognition 118 (2):265-273.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • The role of domain-general cognitive control in language comprehension.Evelina Fedorenko - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  • Language networks: Their structure, function, and evolution.Ricard V. Solé, Bernat Corominas-Murtra, Sergi Valverde & Luc Steels - 2010 - Complexity 15 (6):20-26.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Computational Investigations of Multiword Chunks in Language Learning.Stewart M. McCauley & Morten H. Christiansen - 2017 - Topics in Cognitive Science 9 (3):637-652.
    Second-language learners rarely arrive at native proficiency in a number of linguistic domains, including morphological and syntactic processing. Previous approaches to understanding the different outcomes of first- versus second-language learning have focused on cognitive and neural factors. In contrast, we explore the possibility that children and adults may rely on different linguistic units throughout the course of language learning, with specific focus on the granularity of those units. Following recent psycholinguistic evidence for the role of multiword chunks in online language (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Can Chunk Size Differences Explain Developmental Changes in Lexical Learning?Eleonore H. M. Smalle, Louisa Bogaerts, Morgane Simonis, Wouter Duyck, Michael P. A. Page, Martin G. Edwards & Arnaud Szmalec - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • The resilience of combinatorial structure at the word level: morphology in self-styled gesture systems.Susan Goldin-Meadow, Carolyn Mylander & Cynthia Butcher - 1995 - Cognition 56 (3):195-262.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Principles that are invoked in the acquisition of words, but not facts.Sandra R. Waxman & Amy E. Booth - 2000 - Cognition 77 (2):B33-B43.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  • Bayesian analogy with relational transformations.Hongjing Lu, Dawn Chen & Keith J. Holyoak - 2012 - Psychological Review 119 (3):617-648.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  • Temporal Attention as a Scaffold for Language Development.Ruth de Diego-Balaguer, Anna Martinez-Alvarez & Ferran Pons - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Successful communication does not drive language development: Evidence from adult homesign.Emily M. Carrigan & Marie Coppola - 2017 - Cognition 158 (C):10-27.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • How Does the Mind Work? Insights from Biology.Gary Marcus - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (1):145-172.
    Cognitive scientists must understand not just what the mind does, but how it does what it does. In this paper, I consider four aspects of cognitive architecture: how the mind develops, the extent to which it is or is not modular, the extent to which it is or is not optimal, and the extent to which it should or should not be considered a symbol‐manipulating device (as opposed to, say, an eliminative connectionist network). In each case, I argue that insights (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Bootstrapping the lexicon: a computational model of infant speech segmentation.Eleanor Olds Batchelder - 2002 - Cognition 83 (2):167-206.
    Prelinguistic infants must find a way to isolate meaningful chunks from the continuous streams of speech that they hear. BootLex, a new model which uses distributional cues to build a lexicon, demonstrates how much can be accomplished using this single source of information. This conceptually simple probabilistic algorithm achieves significant segmentation results on various kinds of language corpora - English, Japanese, and Spanish; child- and adult-directed speech, and written texts; and several variations in coding structure - and reveals which statistical (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   12 citations  
  • Children and Adults as Language Learners: Rules, Variation, and Maturational Change.Elissa L. Newport - 2020 - Topics in Cognitive Science 12 (1):153-169.
    Newport addresses a fundamental question in language learning: When, why, and how do learners come to form rules, given linguistic input that varies probabilistically? She presents several case studies that confirm and extend a long‐standing theme of her work: that young learners tend to form rules from variable input, whereas adult learners store and use its statistical probabilities. Thus, child and adult learners use quite different kinds of computations when learning language; the consequence is that operating on the very same (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Adult Learning and Language Simplification.Mark Atkinson, Kenny Smith & Simon Kirby - 2018 - Cognitive Science 42 (8):2818-2854.
    Languages spoken in larger populations are relatively simple. A possible explanation for this is that languages with a greater number of speakers tend to also be those with higher proportions of non‐native speakers, who may simplify language during learning. We assess this explanation for the negative correlation between population size and linguistic complexity in three experiments, using artificial language learning techniques to investigate both the simplifications made by individual adult learners and the potential for such simplifications to influence group‐level language (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Easy Words: Reference Resolution in a Malevolent Referent World.Lila R. Gleitman & John C. Trueswell - 2018 - Topics in Cognitive Science 12 (1):22-47.
    Gleitman and Trueswell’s “The easy words” forms a pair with their earlier paper, “Hard words,” completing a circle in which the authors ask how “easy” words (e.g., concrete nouns) are learned. They take up the hypothesis of “cross‐situational learning,” and argue that accumulating observations actually hinders learning if the mechanism requires holding all exemplars in memory over time. They present an alternative hypothesis, “Propose but Verify,” wherein people use one‐trial learning to confirm or disconfirm their current hypothesis—a mechanism distinctly different (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Visual imagery and visual-spatial language: Enhanced imagery abilities in deaf and hearing ASL signers.Karen Emmorey, Stephen M. Kosslyn & Ursula Bellugi - 1993 - Cognition 46 (2):139-181.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • The development of relative clauses in spontaneous child speech.Holger Diessel & Michael Tomasello - 2001 - Cognitive Linguistics 11 (1-2).
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  • Structure emerges faster during cultural transmission in children than in adults.Vera Kempe, Nicolas Gauvrit & Douglas Forsyth - 2015 - Cognition 136:247-254.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Experiential Limitation in Judgment and Decision.Ulrike Hahn - 2014 - Topics in Cognitive Science 6 (2):229-244.
    The statistics of small samples are often quite different from those of large samples, and this needs to be taken into account in assessing the rationality of human behavior. Specifically, in evaluating human responses to environmental statistics, it is the effective environment that matters; that is, the environment actually experienced by the agent needs to be considered, not simply long‐run frequencies. Significant deviations from long‐run statistics may arise through experiential limitations of the agent that stem from resource constraints and/or information‐processing (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Reproducing American Sign Language sentences: cognitive scaffolding in working memory.Ted Supalla, Peter C. Hauser & Daphne Bavelier - 2014 - Frontiers in Psychology 5:82875.
    The American Sign Language Sentence Reproduction Test (ASL-SRT) requires the precise reproduction of a series of ASL sentences increasing in complexity and length. Error analyses of such tasks provides insight into working memory and scaffolding processes. Data was collected from three groups expected to differ in fluency: deaf children, deaf adults and hearing adults, all users of ASL. Quantitative (correct/incorrect recall) and qualitative error analyses were performed. Percent correct on the reproduction task supports its sensitivity to fluency as test performance (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Statistical learning is constrained to less abstract patterns in complex sensory input.Lauren L. Emberson & Dani Y. Rubinstein - 2016 - Cognition 153 (C):63-78.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Lexical access in Catalan Signed Language (LSC) production.Cristina Baus, Eva Gutiérrez-Sigut, Josep Quer & Manuel Carreiras - 2008 - Cognition 108 (3):856-865.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations