Switch to: References

Add citations

You must login to add citations.
  1. Trade-Off vs. Common Factor—Differentiating Resource-Based Explanations From Their Alternative.Christoph Naefgen & Robert Gaschler - 2022 - Frontiers in Psychology 13:774938.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Acquiring Complex Communicative Systems: Statistical Learning of Language and Emotion.Ashley L. Ruba, Seth D. Pollak & Jenny R. Saffran - 2022 - Topics in Cognitive Science 14 (3):432-450.
    In this article, we consider infants’ acquisition of foundational aspects of language and emotion through the lens of statistical learning. By taking a comparative developmental approach, we highlight ways in which the learning problems presented by input from these two rich communicative domains are both similar and different. Our goal is to encourage other scholars to consider multiple domains of human experience when developing theories in developmental cognitive science.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Infants' developing sensitivity to native language phonotactics: A meta-analysis.Megha Sundara, Z. L. Zhou, Canaan Breiss, Hironori Katsuda & Jeremy Steffman - 2022 - Cognition 221 (C):104993.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Semantics without semantic content.Daniel W. Harris - 2020 - Mind and Language 37 (3):304-328.
    I argue that semantics is the study of the proprietary database of a centrally inaccessible and informationally encapsulated input–output system. This system’s role is to encode and decode partial and defeasible evidence of what speakers are saying. Since information about nonlinguistic context is therefore outside the purview of semantic processing, a sentence’s semantic value is not its content but a partial and defeasible constraint on what it can be used to say. I show how to translate this thesis into a (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   18 citations  
  • Can resources save rationality? ‘Anti-Bayesian’ updating in cognition and perception.Eric Mandelbaum, Isabel Won, Steven Gross & Chaz Firestone - 2020 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 143:e16.
    Resource rationality may explain suboptimal patterns of reasoning; but what of “anti-Bayesian” effects where the mind updates in a direction opposite the one it should? We present two phenomena — belief polarization and the size-weight illusion — that are not obviously explained by performance- or resource-based constraints, nor by the authors’ brief discussion of reference repulsion. Can resource rationality accommodate them?
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • American Sign Language Syntax and Analogical Reasoning Skills Are Influenced by Early Acquisition and Age of Entry to Signing Schools for the Deaf.Jon Henner, Catherine L. Caldwell-Harris, Rama Novogrodsky & Robert Hoffmeister - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • (1 other version)Balancing Effort and Information Transmission During Language Acquisition: Evidence From Word Order and Case Marking.Maryia Fedzechkina, Elissa L. Newport & T. Florian Jaeger - 2017 - Cognitive Science 41 (2):416-446.
    Across languages of the world, some grammatical patterns have been argued to be more common than expected by chance. These are sometimes referred to as (statistical) language universals. One such universal is the correlation between constituent order freedom and the presence of a case system in a language. Here, we explore whether this correlation can be explained by a bias to balance production effort and informativity of cues to grammatical function. Two groups of learners were presented with miniature artificial languages (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  • Faulty rationale for the two factors that dissociate learning systems.Hiroshi Nagata - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):412-413.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Criteria for implicit learning: Deemphasize conscious access, emphasize amnesia.Carol Augart Seger - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):421-422.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Procedural-Memory, Working-Memory, and Declarative-Memory Skills Are Each Associated With Dimensional Integration in Sound-Category Learning.Carolyn Quam, Alisa Wang, W. Todd Maddox, Kimberly Golisch & Andrew Lotto - 2018 - Frontiers in Psychology 9.
    This paper investigates relationships between procedural-memory, declarative-memory, and working-memory skills and adult native English speakers’ novel sound-category learning. Participants completed a sound-categorization task that required integrating two dimensions: one native (vowel quality), one non-native (pitch). Similar information-integration category structures in the visual and auditory domains have been shown to be best learned implicitly (e.g., Maddox, Ing, & Lauritzen, 2006). Thus, we predicted that individuals with greater procedural-memory capacity would better learn sound categories, because procedural memory appears to support implicit learning (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Temporal Attention as a Scaffold for Language Development.Ruth de Diego-Balaguer, Anna Martinez-Alvarez & Ferran Pons - 2016 - Frontiers in Psychology 7.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Can Chunk Size Differences Explain Developmental Changes in Lexical Learning?Eleonore H. M. Smalle, Louisa Bogaerts, Morgane Simonis, Wouter Duyck, Michael P. A. Page, Martin G. Edwards & Arnaud Szmalec - 2015 - Frontiers in Psychology 6.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  • Implementational constraints on human learning and memory systems.Chad J. Marsolek - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):411-412.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • (1 other version)Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems.David R. Shanks & Mark F. St John - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):367-395.
    A number of ways of taxonomizing human learning have been proposed. We examine the evidence for one such proposal, namely, that there exist independent explicit and implicit learning systems. This combines two further distinctions, between learning that takes place with versus without concurrent awareness, and between learning that involves the encoding of instances versus the induction of abstract rules or hypotheses. Implicit learning is assumed to involve unconscious rule learning. We examine the evidence for implicit learning derived from subliminal learning, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   186 citations  
  • Experiential Limitation in Judgment and Decision.Ulrike Hahn - 2014 - Topics in Cognitive Science 6 (2):229-244.
    The statistics of small samples are often quite different from those of large samples, and this needs to be taken into account in assessing the rationality of human behavior. Specifically, in evaluating human responses to environmental statistics, it is the effective environment that matters; that is, the environment actually experienced by the agent needs to be considered, not simply long‐run frequencies. Significant deviations from long‐run statistics may arise through experiential limitations of the agent that stem from resource constraints and/or information‐processing (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The impact of adjacent-dependencies and staged-input on the learnability of center-embedded hierarchical structures.Jun Lai & Fenna H. Poletiek - 2011 - Cognition 118 (2):265-273.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   17 citations  
  • Visual imagery and visual-spatial language: Enhanced imagery abilities in deaf and hearing ASL signers.Karen Emmorey, Stephen M. Kosslyn & Ursula Bellugi - 1993 - Cognition 46 (2):139-181.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   11 citations  
  • A step too far?Dianne C. Berry - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):397-398.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Autonomy and the nature of the input.Wendy Wilkins - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):638-638.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Logic and language acquisition.F. Lowenthal - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):626-627.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Language acquisition in the absence of proof of absence of experience.David M. W. Powers - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):629-630.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • We need a team of gene-mappers, not principle-provers.Thomas Roeper - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):630-631.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • A premature retreat to nativism.Jeffrey L. Sokolov & Catherine E. Snow - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):635-636.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Alternatives to linguistic arbitrariness.Catherine L. Harris - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):622-623.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Language acquisition in the absence of experience.Stephen Crain - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):597-612.
    A fundamental goal of linguistic theory is to explain how natural languages are acquired. This paper describes some recent findings on how learners acquire syntactic knowledge for which there is little, if any, decisive evidence from the environment. The first section presents several general observations about language acquisition that linguistic theory has tried to explain and discusses the thesis that certain linguistic properties are innate because they appear universally and in the absence of corresponding experience. A third diagnostic for innateness, (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   113 citations  
  • Number our days: Quantifying social evolution.Harry J. Jerison - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):712-713.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Grooming is not the only regulator of primate social interactions.Robert M. Seyfarth & Dorothy L. Cheney - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):717-718.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Group structure and group size among humans and other primates.Linton C. Freeman - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):703-704.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do gossip and lack of grooming make us human?Ilya I. Glezer & Warren G. Kinzey - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):704-705.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Do larger brains mean greater intelligence?R. W. Byrne - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):696-697.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • The Role of Prior Experience in Language Acquisition.Jill Lany, Rebecca L. Gómez & Lou Ann Gerken - 2007 - Cognitive Science 31 (3):481-507.
    Learners exposed to an artificial language recognize its abstract structural regularities when instantiated in a novel vocabulary (e.g., Gómez, Gerken, & Schvaneveldt, 2000; Tunney & Altmann, 2001). We asked whether such sensitivity accelerates subsequent learning, and enables acquisition of more complex structure. In Experiment 1, pre-exposure to a category-induction language of the form aX bY sped subsequent learning when the language is instantiated in a different vocabulary. In Experiment 2, while naíve learners did not acquire an acX bcY language, in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans.R. I. M. Dunbar - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):681-694.
    Group size is a function of relative neocortical volume in nonhuman primates. Extrapolation from this regression equation yields a predicted group size for modern humans very similar to that of certain hunter-gatherer and traditional horticulturalist societies. Groups of similar size are also found in other large-scale forms of contemporary and historical society. Among primates, the cohesion of groups is maintained by social grooming; the time devoted to social grooming is linearly related to group size among the Old World monkeys and (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   214 citations  
  • Varieties of consciousness.Paolo Bartolomeo & Gianfranco Dalla Barba - 2002 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (3):331-332.
    In agreement with some of the ideas expressed by Perruchet & Vinter (P&V), we believe that some phenomena hitherto attributed to processing may in fact reflect a fundamental distinction between direct and reflexive forms of consciousness. This dichotomy, developed by the phenomenological tradition, is substantiated by examples coming from experimental psychology and lesion neuropsychology.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Granularity and the acquisition of grammatical gender: How order-of-acquisition affects what gets learned.Inbal Arnon & Michael Ramscar - 2012 - Cognition 122 (3):292-305.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   27 citations  
  • (1 other version)When did mozart become a mozart? Neurophysiological insight into behavioral genetics.Yuri I. Arshavsky - 2003 - Brain and Mind 4 (3):327-339.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Epistemological strata and the rules of right reason.Robert C. Cummins, Pierre Poirier & Martin Roth - 2004 - Synthese 141 (3):287 - 331.
    It has been commonplace in epistemology since its inception to idealize away from computational resource constraints, i.e., from the constraints of time and memory. One thought is that a kind of ideal rationality can be specified that ignores the constraints imposed by limited time and memory, and that actual cognitive performance can be seen as an interaction between the norms of ideal rationality and the practicalities of time and memory limitations. But a cornerstone of naturalistic epistemology is that normative assessment (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   6 citations  
  • (1 other version)Characteristics of dissociable human learning systems.David R. Shanks & Mark F. St John - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):367-447.
    A number of ways of taxonomizing human learning have been proposed. We examine the evidence for one such proposal, namely, that there exist independent explicit and implicit learning systems. This combines two further distinctions, (1) between learning that takes place with versus without concurrent awareness, and (2) between learning that involves the encoding of instances (or fragments) versus the induction of abstract rules or hypotheses. Implicit learning is assumed to involve unconscious rule learning. We examine the evidence for implicit learning (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   193 citations  
  • Representational trajectories in connectionist learning.Andy Clark - 1994 - Minds and Machines 4 (3):317-32.
    The paper considers the problems involved in getting neural networks to learn about highly structured task domains. A central problem concerns the tendency of networks to learn only a set of shallow (non-generalizable) representations for the task, i.e., to miss the deep organizing features of the domain. Various solutions are examined, including task specific network configuration and incremental learning. The latter strategy is the more attractive, since it holds out the promise of a task-independent solution to the problem. Once we (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Can procedural learning be equated with unconscious learning or rule-based learning?Zoe Kourtzi, Lindsay M. Oliver & Mark A. Gluck - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):408-409.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Learning without awareness: What counts as an appropriate test of learning and of awareness.Sam S. Rakover - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):417-418.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Successful communication does not drive language development: Evidence from adult homesign.Emily M. Carrigan & Marie Coppola - 2017 - Cognition 158 (C):10-27.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Developmental Constraints on Learning Artificial Grammars with Fixed, Flexible and Free Word Order.Iga Nowak & Giosuè Baggio - 2017 - Frontiers in Psychology 8.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  • Exploring some edges: Chunk-and-Pass processing at the very beginning, across representations, and on to action.Rose Maier & Dare Baldwin - 2016 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  • Lexical Categories at the Edge of the Word.Luca Onnis & Morten H. Christiansen - 2008 - Cognitive Science 32 (1):184-221.
    Language acquisition may be one of the most difficult tasks that children face during development. They have to segment words from fluent speech, figure out the meanings of these words, and discover the syntactic constraints for joining them together into meaningful sentences. Over the past couple of decades, computational modeling has emerged as a new paradigm for gaining insights into the mechanisms by which children may accomplish these feats. Unfortunately, many of these models assume a computational complexity and linguistic knowledge (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • The Utility of Cognitive Plausibility in Language Acquisition Modeling: Evidence From Word Segmentation.Lawrence Phillips & Lisa Pearl - 2015 - Cognitive Science 39 (8):1824-1854.
    The informativity of a computational model of language acquisition is directly related to how closely it approximates the actual acquisition task, sometimes referred to as the model's cognitive plausibility. We suggest that though every computational model necessarily idealizes the modeled task, an informative language acquisition model can aim to be cognitively plausible in multiple ways. We discuss these cognitive plausibility checkpoints generally and then apply them to a case study in word segmentation, investigating a promising Bayesian segmentation strategy. We incorporate (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  • Whither learning, whither memory?Michael A. Stadler & Peter A. Frensch - 1994 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):423-424.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  • Can Crain constrain the constraints?Dan I. Slobin - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):633-634.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • Noninnatist alternatives to the negative evidence hypothesis.David Dodd & Alan Fogel - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):617-618.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  • The rest of the story: Grooming, group size and vocal exchanges in neotropical primates.Charles T. Snowdon - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):718-718.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   5 citations  
  • Behavioural constraints on social communication are not likely to prevent the evolution of large social groups in nonhuman primates.R. J. Andrew - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (4):694-694.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark