- Semantics and Cognition.Steven E. Boër - 1985 - Philosophical Review 94 (1):111.details
|
|
Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax: Information processing contributions to sentence formulation.J. Kathryn Bock - 1982 - Psychological Review 89 (1):1-47.details
|
|
Language production and serial order: A functional analysis and a model.Gary S. Dell, Lisa K. Burger & William R. Svec - 1997 - Psychological Review 104 (1):123-147.details
|
|
Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition.John E. Hummel & Irving Biederman - 1992 - Psychological Review 99 (3):480-517.details
|
|
A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production.Gary S. Dell - 1986 - Psychological Review 93 (3):283-321.details
|
|
Making Syntax of Sense: Number Agreement in Sentence Production.Kathleen M. Eberhard, J. Cooper Cutting & Kathryn Bock - 2005 - Psychological Review 112 (3):531-559.details
|
|
Framing sentences.K. Bock - 1990 - Cognition 35 (1):1-39.details
|
|
Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue.Holly P. Branigan, Martin J. Pickering & Alexandra A. Cleland - 2000 - Cognition 75 (2):B13-B25.details
|
|
Do young children have adult syntactic competence?Michael Tomasello - 2000 - Cognition 74 (3):209-253.details
|
|
The role of abstract syntactic knowledge in language acquisition: a reply to Tomasello.Cynthia Fisher - 2002 - Cognition 82 (3):259-278.details
|
|
A theory of lexical access in speech production.Willem J. M. Levelt, Ardi Roelofs & Antje S. Meyer - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (1):1-38.details
|
|
Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis.Jerry A. Fodor & Zenon W. Pylyshyn - 1988 - Cognition 28 (1-2):3-71.details
|
|
Learning and applying contextual constraints in sentence comprehension.Mark F. St John & James L. McClelland - 1990 - Artificial Intelligence 46 (1-2):217-257.details
|
|
Computing the Meanings of Words in Reading: Cooperative Division of Labor Between Visual and Phonological Processes.Michael W. Harm & Mark S. Seidenberg - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (3):662-720.details
|
|
Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind.George Lakoff - 1987 - Philosophy and Rhetoric 22 (4):299-302.details
|
|
(2 other versions)Syntactic Structures.J. F. Staal - 1966 - Journal of Symbolic Logic 31 (2):245-251.details
|
|
Can thematic roles leave traces of their places?Franklin Chang, Kathryn Bock & Adele E. Goldberg - 2003 - Cognition 90 (1):29-49.details
|
|
Incremental interpretation at verbs: restricting the domain of subsequent reference.Gerry T. M. Altmann & Yuki Kamide - 1999 - Cognition 73 (3):247-264.details
|
|
Syntactic structure assembly in human parsing: a computational model based on competitive inhibition and a lexicalist grammar.Theo Vosse & Gerard Kempen - 2000 - Cognition 75 (2):105-143.details
|
|
The consciousness of self.William James - 1890 - In The Principles of Psychology. London, England: Dover Publications.details
|
|
Verbal behavior.Noam Chomsky & B. F. Skinner - 1959 - Language 35 (1):26.details
|
|
Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production.Brenda Rapp & Matthew Goldrick - 2000 - Psychological Review 107 (3):460-499.details
|
|
The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution.Maryellen C. MacDonald, Neal J. Pearlmutter & Mark S. Seidenberg - 1994 - Psychological Review 101 (4):676-703.details
|
|
From conceptual roles to structural relations: Bridging the syntactic cleft.Kathryn Bock, Helga Loebell & Randal Morey - 1992 - Psychological Review 99 (1):150-171.details
|
|
(1 other version)A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming.Mark S. Seidenberg & James L. McClelland - 1989 - Psychological Review 96 (4):523-568.details
|
|
The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation.Steven W. Keele, Richard Ivry, Ulrich Mayr, Eliot Hazeltine & Herbert Heuer - 2003 - Psychological Review 110 (2):316-339.details
|
|
Structure and Deterioration of Semantic Memory: A Neuropsychological and Computational Investigation.Timothy T. Rogers, Matthew A. Lambon Ralph, Peter Garrard, Sasha Bozeat, James L. McClelland, John R. Hodges & Karalyn Patterson - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (1):205-235.details
|
|
Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech.Toben H. Mintz - 2003 - Cognition 90 (1):91-117.details
|
|
From simple associations to systematic reasoning: A connectionist representation of rules, variables, and dynamic binding using temporal synchrony.Lokendra Shastri & Venkat Ajjanagadde - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (3):417-51.details
|
|
Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996).Maryellen C. MacDonald & Morten H. Christiansen - 2002 - Psychological Review 109 (1):35-54.details
|
|
Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains.David C. Plaut, James L. McClelland, Mark S. Seidenberg & Karalyn Patterson - 1996 - Psychological Review 103 (1):56-115.details
|
|
Doing Without Schema Hierarchies: A Recurrent Connectionist Approach to Normal and Impaired Routine Sequential Action.Matthew Botvinick & David C. Plaut - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (2):395-429.details
|
|
Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience.Kyle E. Chambers, Kristine H. Onishi & Cynthia Fisher - 2003 - Cognition 87 (2):B69-B77.details
|
|
Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small.Jeffrey L. Elman - 1993 - Cognition 48 (1):71-99.details
|
|
Learning phonotactic constraints from brief auditory experience.K. Onishi - 2002 - Cognition 83 (1):B13-B23.details
|
|
The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components.P. Indefrey & W. J. M. Levelt - 2003 - Cognition 92 (1-2):101-144.details
|
|
Paradox lost? No, paradox found! Reply to Tomasello and Akhtar.Letitia R. Naigles - 2003 - Cognition 88 (3):325-329.details
|
|
What paradox? A response to Naigles.Michael Tomasello & Nameera Akhtar - 2003 - Cognition 88 (3):317-323.details
|
|
Closed-class immanence in sentence production.Kathryn Bock - 1989 - Cognition 31 (2):163-186.details
|
|
Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: how important is starting small?Douglas L. T. Rohde & David C. Plaut - 1999 - Cognition 72 (1):67-109.details
|
|
Whence and whither in spatial language and spatial cognition?Barbara Landau & Ray Jackendoff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):255-265.details
|
|
The influence of the immediate visual context on incremental thematic role-assignment: evidence from eye-movements in depicted events.Pia Knoeferle, Matthew W. Crocker, Christoph Scheepers & Martin J. Pickering - 2005 - Cognition 95 (1):95-127.details
|
|
Artificial syntactic violations activate Broca's region.K. Petersson - 2004 - Cognitive Science 28 (3):383-407.details
|
|
Syntactic priming of relative clause attachments: persistence of structural configuration in sentence production.Christoph Scheepers - 2003 - Cognition 89 (3):179-205.details
|
|
“What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition.Barbara Landau & Ray Jackendoff - 1993 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16 (2):217-238.details
|
|
The visual brain in action (precis).David Milner - 1998 - PSYCHE: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research On Consciousness 4.details
|
|
Word order priming in written and spoken sentence production.Robert J. Hartsuiker & Casper Westenberg - 2000 - Cognition 75 (2):B27-B39.details
|
|
Understanding how input matters: verb learning and the footprint of universal grammar.Jeffrey Lidz, Henry Gleitman & Lila Gleitman - 2003 - Cognition 87 (3):151-178.details
|
|
A tale of two theories: response to Fisher.Michael Tomasello & Kirsten Abbot-Smith - 2002 - Cognition 83 (2):207-214.details
|
|
Form is easy, meaning is hard: resolving a paradox in early child language.Letitia R. Naigles - 2002 - Cognition 86 (2):157-199.details
|
|