Switch to: Citations

References in:

The intuitive mind

Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17 (3):414-414 (1994)

Add references

You must login to add references.
  1. Consciousness is a “subjective” state.Philip M. Merikle & Jim Cheesman - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):42-42.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   83 citations  
  • An operant analysis of problem solving.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):583-591.
    Behavior that solves a problem is distinguished by the fact that it changes another part of the solver's behavior and is strengthened when it does so. Problem solving typically involves the construction of discriminative stimuli. Verbal responses produce especially useful stimuli, because they affect other people. As a culture formulates maxims, laws, grammar, and science, its members behave more effectively without direct or prolonged contact with the contingencies thus formulated. The culture solves problems for its members, and does so by (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   47 citations  
  • Contingency awareness in evaluative conditioning: A case for unaware affective-evaluative learning.Frank Baeyens, Paul Eelen & Omer van den Bergh - 1990 - Cognition and Emotion 4 (1):3-18.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   65 citations  
  • Contingency awareness in evaluative conditioning: A comment on baeyens, eelen, and van den bergh.David R. Shanks & Anthony Dickinson - 1990 - Cognition and Emotion 4 (1):19-30.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • Verbal control of an autonomic response in a cue reversal situation.William W. Grings, Anne M. Schell & Cheryl A. Carey - 1973 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 99 (2):215.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Transfer of syntactic structure in synthetic languages.Arthur S. Reber - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (1):115.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   82 citations  
  • Judgments of ucs intensity and diminution of the ucr in classical gsr conditioning.Ellen Kimmel - 1967 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 73 (4p1):532.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   37 citations  
  • Learning without awareness of what is being learned or intent to learn it.E. L. Thorndike & R. T. Rock - 1934 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 17 (1):1.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Connectionist and Memory‐Array Models of Artificial Grammar Learning.Zoltan Dienes - 1992 - Cognitive Science 16 (1):41-79.
    Subjects exposed to strings of letters generated by a finite state grammar can later classify grammatical and nongrammatical test strings, even though they cannot adequately say what the rules of the grammar are (e.g., Reber, 1989). The MINERVA 2 (Hintzman, 1986) and Medin and Schaffer (1978) memory‐array models and a number of connectionist outoassociator models are tested against experimental data by deriving mainly parameter‐free predictions from the models of the rank order of classification difficulty of test strings. The importance of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   61 citations  
  • The Case for Rules in Reasoning.Edward E. Smith, Christopher Langston & Richard E. Nisbett - 1992 - Cognitive Science 16 (1):1-40.
    A number of theoretical positions in psychology—including variants of case‐based reasoning, instance‐based analogy, and connectionist models—maintain that abstract rules are not involved in human reasoning, or at best play a minor role. Other views hold that the use of abstract rules is a core aspect of human reasoning. We propose eight criteria for determining whether or not people use abstract rules in reasoning, and examine evidence relevant to each criterion for several rule systems. We argue that there is substantial evidence (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   75 citations  
  • On the genesis of abstract ideas.M. I. Posner & S. W. Keele - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 77 (2p1):353-363.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   165 citations  
  • Maturational Constraints on Language Learning.Elissa L. Newport - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (1):11-28.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   203 citations  
  • (1 other version)Finding Structure in Time.Jeffrey L. Elman - 1990 - Cognitive Science 14 (2):179-211.
    Time underlies many interesting human behaviors. Thus, the question of how to represent time in connectionist models is very important. One approach is to represent time implicitly by its effects on processing rather than explicitly (as in a spatial representation). The current report develops a proposal along these lines first described by Jordan (1986) which involves the use of recurrent links in order to provide networks with a dynamic memory. In this approach, hidden unit patterns are fed back to themselves: (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   511 citations  
  • (1 other version)Implicit memory: Retention without remembering.Henry L. Roediger - 1990 - American Psychologist 45:1043-1056.
    Explicit measures of human memory, such as recall or recognition, reflect conscious recollection of the past. Implicit tests of retention measure transfer (or priming) from past experience on tasks that do not require conscious recollection of recent experiences for their performance. The article reviews research on the relation between explicit and implicit memory. The evidence points to substantial differences between standard explicit and implicit tests, because many variables create dissociations between these tests. For example, although pictures are remembered better than (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   119 citations  
  • Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information.James L. McClelland & David E. Rumelhart - 1985 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 114 (2):159-188.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   185 citations  
  • Context theory of classification learning.Douglas L. Medin & Marguerite M. Schaffer - 1978 - Psychological Review 85 (3):207-238.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   402 citations  
  • Empirical criteria for task susceptibility to introspective awareness and awareness effects.Sam Rakover - 1993 - Philosophical Psychology 6 (4):451 – 467.
    A proposed empirical criterion for task susceptibility to introspective awareness distinguishes cognitive processes of which one cannot be aware from those of which one can be aware. The empirical criterion for task susceptibility to awareness effects proposes that there are tasks which cannot be affected by awareness of the rules constituting the tasks. These criteria were applied to research programmes in rule-learning in which past studies in the area of learning without awareness were included as well as current research in (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   39 citations  
  • Behaviorism at fifty.B. F. Skinner - 1974 - New York,: J. Norton Publishers.
    Each of us is uniquely subject to certain kinds of stimulation from a small part of the universe within our skins. Mentalistic psychologies insist that other kinds of events, lacking the physical dimensions of stimuli, are accessible to the owner of the skin within which they occur. One solution often regarded as behavioristic, granting the distinction between public and private events and ruling the latter out of consideration, has not been successful. A science of behavior must face the problem of (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   51 citations  
  • Taking phenomenology seriously: The "fringe" and its implication for cognitive research.Bruce Mangan - 1993 - Consciousness and Cognition 2 (2):89-108.
    Evidence and theory ranging from traditional philosophy to contemporary cognitive research support the hypothesis that consciousness has a two-part structure: a focused region of articulated experience surrounded by a field of relatively unarticulated, vague experience.William James developed an especially useful phenomenological analysis of this "fringe" of consciousness, but its relation to, and potential value for, the study of cognition has not been explored. I propose strengthening James′ work on the fringe with a functional analysis: fringe experiences work to radically condense (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   103 citations  
  • (1 other version)Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes.Richard E. Nisbett & Timothy D. Wilson - 1977 - Psychological Review 84 (3):231-59.
    Reviews evidence which suggests that there may be little or no direct introspective access to higher order cognitive processes. Ss are sometimes unaware of the existence of a stimulus that importantly influenced a response, unaware of the existence of the response, and unaware that the stimulus has affected the response. It is proposed that when people attempt to report on their cognitive processes, that is, on the processes mediating the effects of a stimulus on a response, they do not do (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1521 citations  
  • An evolutionary context for the cognitive unconscious.Arthur S. Reber - 1992 - Philosophical Psychology 5 (1):33-51.
    This paper is an attempt to put the work of the past several decades on the problems of implicit learning and unconscious cognition into an evolutionary context. Implicit learning is an inductive process whereby knowledge of a complex environment is acquired and used largely independently of awareness of either the process of acquisition or the nature of that which has been learned. Characterized this way, implicit learning theory can be viewed as an attempt to come to grips with the classic (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • Learning and consciousness during general anesthesia.M. M. Ghoneim & R. I. Block - 1992 - Anesthesiology 76:279-305.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • Learning during anesthesia: A review.Jackie Andrade - 1995 - British Journal of Psychology 86:479-506.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious.Arthur S. Reber - 1993 - Oxford University Press.
    In this new volume in the Oxford Psychology Series, the author presents a highly readable account of the cognitive unconscious, focusing in particular on the problem of implicit learning. Implicit learning is defined as the acquisition of knowledge that takes place independently of the conscious attempts to learn and largely in the absence of explicit knowledge about what was acquired. One of the core assumptions of this argument is that implicit learning is a fundamental, "root" process, one that lies at (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   187 citations  
  • On the Relation Between Memory and Consciousness: Dissociable Interactions and Conscious Experience. In (H. Roediger & F.Daniel L. Schacter - 1989 - In Henry L. I. Roediger & Fergus I. M. Craik (eds.), Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays in Honor of Endel Tulving. Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   41 citations  
  • Perception without awareness: Critical issues.Philip M. Merikle - 1992 - American Psychologist 47:792-5.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   76 citations  
  • Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal.Daniel Holender - 1986 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 9 (1):1-23.
    When the stored representation of the meaning of a stimulus is accessed through the processing of a sensory input it is maintained in an activated state for a certain amount of time that allows for further processing. This semantic activation is generally accompanied by conscious identification, which can be demonstrated by the ability of a person to perform discriminations on the basis of the meaning of the stimulus. The idea that a sensory input can give rise to semantic activation without (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   449 citations  
  • Discrimination and learning without awareness: A metholodological survey and evaluation.Charles W. Eriksen - 1960 - Psychological Review 67 (5):279-300.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   186 citations  
  • Subliminal mere exposure effects.Robert F. Bornstein - 1992 - In Robert F. Bornstein & Thane S. Pittman (eds.), Perception Without Awareness: Cognitive, Clinical, and Social Perspectives. Guilford.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   52 citations  
  • (2 other versions)Controlled and automatic human information processing: Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory.Richard M. Shiffrin & Walter Schneider - 1977 - Psychological Review 84 (2):128-90.
    Tested the 2-process theory of detection, search, and attention presented by the current authors in a series of experiments. The studies demonstrate the qualitative difference between 2 modes of information processing: automatic detection and controlled search; trace the course of the learning of automatic detection, of categories, and of automatic-attention responses; and show the dependence of automatic detection on attending responses and demonstrate how such responses interrupt controlled processing and interfere with the focusing of attention. The learning of categories is (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   804 citations  
  • Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action.Benjamin Libet - 1985 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8 (4):529-66.
    Voluntary acts are preceded by electrophysiological (RPs). With spontaneous acts involving no preplanning, the main negative RP shift begins at about200 ms. Control experiments, in which a skin stimulus was timed (S), helped evaluate each subject's error in reporting the clock times for awareness of any perceived event.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   762 citations  
  • (2 other versions)The operational analysis of psychological terms.B. F. Skinner - 1945 - Psychological Review 52 (4):270-78.
    The major contributions of operationism have been negative, largely because operationists failed to distinguish logical theories of reference from empirical accounts of language. Behaviorism never finished an adequate formulation of verbal reports and therefore could not convincingly embrace subjective terms. But verbal responses to private stimuli can arise as social products through the contingencies of reinforcement arranged by verbal communities.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   105 citations  
  • Is human information processing conscious?Max Velmans - 1991 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14 (4):651-69.
    Investigations of the function of consciousness in human information processing have focused mainly on two questions: (1) where does consciousness enter into the information processing sequence and (2) how does conscious processing differ from preconscious and unconscious processing. Input analysis is thought to be initially "preconscious," "pre-attentive," fast, involuntary, and automatic. This is followed by "conscious," "focal-attentive" analysis which is relatively slow, voluntary, and flexible. It is thought that simple, familiar stimuli can be identified preconsciously, but conscious processing is needed (...)
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   348 citations  
  • (5 other versions)What is it like to be a bat?Thomas Nagel - 1974 - Philosophical Review 83 (4):435-50.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2254 citations  
  • Lectures on Contemporary Syntactic Theories: An Introduction to Government- Binding Theory, Generalized Phrase Structure Grammar, and Lexical- Functional Grammar.Peter Sells - 1989 - Journal of Symbolic Logic 54 (2):628-630.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   43 citations  
  • Learning and applying contextual constraints in sentence comprehension.Mark F. St John & James L. McClelland - 1990 - Artificial Intelligence 46 (1-2):217-257.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   94 citations  
  • (1 other version)Behaviorism at fifty.B. F. Skinner - 1984 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (4):615.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   45 citations  
  • (1 other version)The Concept of Mind.Gilbert Ryle - 1949 - Revue Philosophique de la France Et de l'Etranger 141:125-126.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2261 citations  
  • (2 other versions)The Principles of Psychology.William James - 1890 - The Monist 1:284.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1643 citations  
  • Verbal reports as data.K. Anders Ericsson & Herbert A. Simon - 1980 - Psychological Review 87 (3):215-251.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   257 citations  
  • An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings.James L. McClelland & David E. Rumelhart - 1981 - Psychological Review 88 (5):375-407.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   633 citations  
  • (2 other versions)Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention.Walter Schneider & Richard M. Shiffrin - 1977 - Psychological Review 84 (1):1-66.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   592 citations  
  • Retrieval independence in recognition and recall.Arthur J. Flexser & Endel Tulving - 1978 - Psychological Review 85 (3):153-171.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   124 citations  
  • Descriptive behaviorism versus cognitive theory in verbal operant conditioning.Charles D. Spielberger & L. Douglas DeNike - 1966 - Psychological Review 73 (4):306-326.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   39 citations  
  • Dissociated overt and covert recognition as an emergent property of a lesioned neural network.Martha J. Farah, Randall C. O'Reilly & Shaun P. Vecera - 1993 - Psychological Review 100 (4):571-588.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   141 citations  
  • On measuring (in)dependence of cognitive processes.Mark L. Howe, F. Michael Rabinowitz & Malcolm J. Grant - 1993 - Psychological Review 100 (4):737-747.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   38 citations  
  • (1 other version)ALCOVE: An exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning.John K. Kruschke - 1992 - Psychological Review 99 (1):22-44.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   223 citations  
  • Rule-plus-exception model of classification learning.Robert M. Nosofsky, Thomas J. Palmeri & Stephen C. McKinley - 1994 - Psychological Review 101 (1):53-79.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   126 citations  
  • (1 other version)A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming.Mark S. Seidenberg & James L. McClelland - 1989 - Psychological Review 96 (4):523-568.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   418 citations  
  • (1 other version)Toward an instance theory of automatization.Gordon D. Logan - 1988 - Psychological Review 95 (4):492-527.
    Download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   252 citations